TYPE Original Research
PUBLISHED 18 January 2023
DOI 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Conrad Ziller,
University of Duisburg-Essen,
Germany
REVIEWED BY
Norman Sempijja,
Mohammed VI Polytechnic University,
Morocco
Elizabeth J. Zechmeister,
Vanderbilt University, United States
Paul Vierus,
University of Duisburg-Essen,
Germany
*CORRESPONDENCE
Felix Jäger
SPECIALTY SECTION
This article was submitted to
Political Participation,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Political Science
RECEIVED 29 July 2022
ACCEPTED 28 December 2022
PUBLISHED 18 January 2023
CITATION
Jäger F (2023) Security vs. civil
liberties: How citizens cope with
threat, restriction, and ideology.
Front. Polit. Sci. 4:1006711.
doi: 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
COPYRIGHT
© 2023 Jäger. This is an open-access
article distributed under the terms of
the
Creative Commons Attribution
License (CC BY)
. The use, distribution
or reproduction in other forums is
permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s)
are credited and that the original
publication in this journal is cited, in
accordance with accepted academic
practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does
not comply with these terms.
Security vs. civil liberties: How
citizens cope with threat,
restriction, and ideology
Felix Jäger
*
Mannheim Centre for European Social Research, Graduate School of Social Sciences, University of
Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
How do citizens balance their preferences for civil liberties and security
in the context of a competitive party system? Using the case of terrorism
and counter-terrorism, I argue that the willingness to support restrictions of
civil liberties does not only depend on external shocks and being targeted
by a counter-policy. Instead, it also depends on their ideological match
with policymakers and terrorist actors. Using an original survey experiment
conducted in Germany in 2022, I study how the four factors feeling threatened
by a terrorist attack, being targeted by a surveillance measure, the ideology
behind an attack, and the partisanship of counteracting politicians influence
the attitudes of citizens and whether these factors are mu tually dependent.
While earlier research has focused on one kind of terrorism (mostly Islamic),
this paper examines various forms of terrorism (religious, right-wing, and
climate-radical) and how they aect peoples’ attitudes toward civil liberties
and surveillance. The results show that terrorist ideology plays a minor
role, but that it matters whether citizens sympathize with the party that
proposes a policy. The study extends our understanding of the political
consequences of polarization, threat perceptions of terrorism, and public
support for surveillance policies.
KEYWORDS
security, civil liberties, terrorism, ideology, polarization, policy preferences,
surveillance, survey experiment
1. Introduction
In times of crisis, civil liberties often have to be restricted for a higher good. Since
civil liberties are one of the great accomplishments of democracy, this is not an easy
decision for governments to make or for citizens to support. When citizens are asked
how important democratic values such as civil liberties are to them, they rate them very
high (Sullivan and Hendriks, 2009). However, these rights are not set in stone and cannot
be considered in a vacuum (
Peffley et al., 2001; Jenkins-Smith and Herron, 2009; Graham
and Svolik, 2020
), as they entail trade-offs with other, highly valuable rights. One of the
strongest conflicts is that between security and civil liberties. This applies to different
external shocks, such as a pandemic, war, or terrorism (
MacKuen and Brown, 1987;
Rohde and Rohde, 2011).
Frontiers in Political Science 01 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
Policymakers react to these external shocks by implementing
policies to protect the population from such dangers. These
policies are a materialization of the norm conflict between civil
liberties and security. However, these policies are often heavily
debated both within parliaments and among the general public.
In these discussions, not only the content of the policies matters,
but also the ideology of the parties who are proposing them.
In this paper I argue that the willingness to support
restrictions of civil liberties depends not only on external
shocks, but also depends on whether citizens are inclined or
averse to policymakers. The acceptance of opinions from other
people or actors who have an opposing ideology or partisanship
is limited within a competitive party system or a polarized
society. Polarization along part y lines is no new phenomenon,
but the level of polarization has increased over the last two
decades (
Heltzel and Laurin, 2020; Druckman et al., 2021). In
Germany, where this s tudy is conducted, affective polarization
has been slowly increasing since 2008 (
Harteveld and Wagner,
2022
). A major driver of polarization has been the far-right
party Alternative für Deutschland (Alternative for Germany,
short AfD) since its foundation in 2013 (
Siri, 2018). Affective
polarization in Germany is mainly between partisans of the AfD
and other partisans. This disliking is asymmetrical, with a higher
aversion of supporters from other parties toward the AfD. AfD
supporters, in contrast, are less negative toward other partisans
(
Jungkunz, 2021).
Consequences of polarization along partisanship can be
seen, for example, in studies about democratic backsliding
(
Somer and McCoy, 2018; Svolik, 2019). The results concerning
citizens propensity to favor partisanship over democratic norms
are mixed.
Carey et al. (2022) find that citizens are defending
democratic norms even when this requires a punishment of
a candidate from the own camp. Other studies find opposing
results, according to which partis anship is valued more highly
than democratic norms (
Graham and Svolik, 2020; Kawecki,
2022
; Saikkonen and Christensen, 2022). The polarization along
partisanship embeds the norm conflict of civil liberties and
security in a societal context. This constitutes the first research
gap this paper is addressing. The paper is guided by the question:
How do citizens balance their preferences for civil liberties and
security in the context of a competitive party system?
A case in which the necessity occurs to find a balance
between civil liberties and security is terrorism and counter-
terrorism as a reaction to it. Looking at specific cases is
necessary, as they add additional elements and variables for
citizens to consider. In the case of terrorism, the major
explaining factor is perceived threat. In general, the higher the
perceived threat, the higher the support for security even at
the expense of civil liberties (
Huddy et al., 2005, 2007; Haider-
Markel et al., 2006
). A second element is the ideology or
motivation of terrorists (
Caton and Mullinix, 2022). Not every
motivation generates the fear or perceived threat of becoming
a target in every citizen equally. A white person might feel less
threatened by right-wing terrorism than a non-white person,
so as an ordinary citizen might feel less threatened by left-
wing terrorism than a person in a leading position. Turning this
relationship around, the motivation of terrorists could even lead
citizens to support them. However, most studies focus on a single
type of terrorism, which has been mainly Islamist terrorism
in the last two decades. Despite right-wing terrorism being
responsible for many attacks in western democracies, especially
in Germany and the U.S.
1
, only few articles have so far looked at
different kinds of terrorism (
Pronin et al., 2006; Wynter, 2017).
This study addresses this second research gap by comparing
how different terrorist motivations (Islamist, right-wing and
climate-radical) influence citizens preferences for security.
To answer the research question, the four stated elements
(feeling threatened by a terrorist attack, being targeted by a
surveillance measure, the ideology behind an attack, and the
partisanship of counteracting politicians) are considered. Using
a survey experiment allows me to vary t hese elements through
specific treatments and to compare citizens policy preferences
under t hese conditions. Such a design complements natural
experiments (
Bozzoli and Müller, 2011; Giani et al., 2021),
which are limited to actual attacks and cannot exclude external
circumstances. The perception of actual terrorist attacks can
be influenced by other simultaneous events, such as election
campaigns (Muñoz et al., 2020). These limitations can be
overcome by the survey experiment employed here. The design
has the practical advantage that, for example, the influence
of different terrorist motivations on citizens’ attitudes can be
examined, which would not be possible in the real world.
The survey experiment was pre-registered and conducted in
Germany in 2022.
The results show that, first, citizens value their privacy and
prefer targeted measures. Second, partisanship matters even in
a crisis. Citizens are willing to accept cuts in civil liberties when
they are proposed by their preferred party. When these cuts are
proposed by a disliked party, the support decreases strongly.
This logic does not apply to terrorist motivation. Citizens do not
change their support for civil liberties when terrorist attacks are
motivated by an extreme form of an ideology t he citizens adhere
to. This result holds for citizens with a high level of extremism
and citizens with a high propensity to violence.
The study expands previous findings by looking at the
conflict of civil liberties and security through the lens of
partisanship and ideology. In times of crisis, when difficult
decisions must be made, parties should work together to gain
support from the population. The good news for societies is that
the political affiliation of citizens does not extend to support for
ideologically close terrorist attacks.
Increasing our knowledge about citizens preferences for
security and civil liberties is crucial for western democracies.
1 https://www.csis.org/analysis/escalating-terrorism-problem-
united-states
Frontiers in Political Science 02 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
TABLE 1 Four combinations of ordinary citizens who experience an
external shock and are targeted by a counter-policy.
External shock
Personal
threat
No personal
threat
Counter-policy Not being
targeted
A B
Being
targeted
C D
While security has been treated as a higher good by politicians
in recent years (
Hegemann and Kahl, 2018), the attitudes of
citizens should not be ignored. Undermining civil liberties
extensively can lead to undesirable developments. A restriction
of fundamental freedom can be the beginning of democratic
backsliding. While this is not a fast, overnight process, it can
open paths that turn away from democracy or facilitate ongoing
processes. A better understanding of the circumstances or
reasons why citizens support restrictions of civil liberties opens
up the possibility of counteracting such movements or seeking
other solutions to strengthen democracy where it is needed.
2. Literature and arguments
2.1. Threat and surveillance—the
trade-o between security and civil
liberties
One aspect of citizens’ decision to support a policy is the
calculation of whether the policy improves their situation or
not. It comes down to the personal situation of citizens. In
the context of civil liberties and security, citizens have to ask
themselves whether t he policy is increasing security more than
civil liberties are restricted. This has to be balanced against the
incoming threat, which should be prevented by the policy. The
relationship c an be applied to any deb ate about civil liberties vs.
security, for example health protection issues during a pandemic
or the prevention of terrorism.
Table 1 shows the stated matrix between an external shock
and the counter-policy. Using the specific case of terrorist threat
and counter-terrorism policies, I explain in the following how
these two factors are expected to influence citizens preference
for security and civil liberties.
Threat is arguably the strongest predictor and best-examined
factor in studies about citizens preferences in the context of
terrorism. A large body of literature on support for security
policies exists that examines the predicting effect of perceived
threat exclusively or among other factors (
Huddy et al., 2002,
2005; Davis and Silver, 2004; Hetherington and Suhay, 2011;
Asbrock and Fritsche, 2013; Cohen-Louck, 2019; Breznau,
2021
). Unfortunately, the nomenclature is not consistent across
studies (
Feldman, 2013, p. 55). Threat, perceived threat, or
the perception of risk describe the same issue from a slightly
different angle, but these terms are often used interchangeably.
In this study, I define perceived threat as an outcome of
an external shock, event, or situation the individual citizen
is confronted with and which is interpreted or perceived as
negative or dangerous. This definition focuses on perceived
personal threat and not on societal, sociotropic , or national
threat.
Perceived personal threat directly concerns individuals
confronted with the external shock. In such a situation,
individuals “will probably be made particularly aware of their
own vulnerability” (
Trüdinger, 2019, p. 37). This awareness
of becoming a victim leads people to think more about their
in-group than about themselves as individuals (
Asbrock and
Fritsche, 2013
). This awareness and the wish for protection for
oneself and the own in-group translates to policy preferences
for security. In the case of terrorism, it seems very likely
that personally threatened citizens will favor security over civil
liberties (
Hetherington and Suhay, 2011).
These external shocks (or, more specific, terrorist attacks),
which are a personal threat for individuals, have to be separated
from a general or omnipresent fear of terrorist attacks. A general
fear of external shocks is an individual predisposition, which
describes citizens general sensitivity to threat or baseline threat
(
Marcus et al., 1995, p. 107). An individual who has a higher
baseline threat is expected to have a higher preference for
security in general. For example, more fearful individuals are
more in favor of restrictive migration policies (
Helbling et al.,
2022
).
The individual shocks provide a “contemporary
information” (
Marcus et al., 1995, p. 107), which provokes
the perception of threat. As
Trüdinger (2019, p. 35) put it,
“[T]he consequences of perceived thre at can be considerable as
it mig ht result in a complete change in political reasoning”. In
danger of an external shock, “people try to restore perceptions
of global control over their environment, which are at
stake in times of threat” (
Fritsche et al., 2011, p. 102). This
regain of control is expressed as an increased preference for
security me asures.
H1a: Citizens suppor t for counter-terrorism measures
increases when they are personally threatened by
terrorist attacks.
The rows in
Table 1 show the counter-policy. The rationale
behind the influence of this dimension on citizens’ policy
support is similar to the influence of the external shock. As
in the external shock dimension, citizen can also be target of
the counter-policy or not. The possibility of this differentiation
depends on the individual measure because not all counter-
policies to external shocks concern the ordinary citizen.
The term counter-terrorism covers a lot of different policies,
ranging from military actions abroad (
Gadarian, 2010) to
Frontiers in Political Science 03 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
immigration regulations (Helbling and Meierrieks, 2020) and
domestic measures such as surveillance (
van Leeuwen, 2003;
Ziller and Helbling, 2021). In this paper, I operationalize
counter-terrorism as a surveillance policy. Surveillance can
target suspicious individuals or groups, which would not
concern ordinary citizens. Such a measure is rather easy for
citizens to support, since it does not impose any restrictions on
them. This is similar to other measures, for example a policy
aiming to disrupt financial flows of terror ists. These measures do
not affect ordinary citizens and thus come at no personal cost.
In contrast, dragnet surveillance affects every citizen in
the state, so citizens are indirectly targeted. In this case,
the measure entails the conflict between security and civil
liberties (
Kossowska et al., 2011). Security should be increased
by preventing terrorist acts. Civil liberties are restricted, as
surveillance can severely curtail privacy rights (
Ziller and
Helbling, 2021
). Surveillance falls in the category of privacy
laws and directly affects citizens in contrast to measures that
concern procedural or immigration laws (
Epifanio, 2011). Since
citizens are expected to value their privacy, dragnet measures
should receive less support than measures targeting suspect
individuals.
H1b: Citizens support for counter-terrorism
measures decreases when they are targeted by the
counter-terrorism policy.
The expectations stated in H1a and H1b are rather
straightforward: respondents to whom case A in
Table 1 applies
should be most supportive of a security measure, respondents
in case D the least suppor tive. Case C is the most interesting
case, as citizens are confronted with the dilemma of threat
and restriction of civil liberties. Depending on the measures
implemented by the government, the individual liberties are
curtailed (
Davis and Silver, 2004). The theoretical argument
in the literature on perceived threat highlights the strength of
the influence that perceived threat has on individuals’ attitudes.
Following this line of research, I argue that respondents strongly
support security measures even at the cost of their personal
liberties.
H1c: The feeling of being threatened by a terrorist attack
outweighs the feeling of being targeted by a policy and drives
the support for a counter-terrorism measure.
2.2. How ideology and partisanship
influence citizens’ willingness to favor
security over civil liberties
While the need to balance these two factors is very clear, t he
support for policies has to be examined in the context of a society
in which citizens have different ideological stances. The ne cessity
to do so becomes clear by going b ack to
Table 1. Bot h sides of the
table can be extended by an ideological dimension. Threat when
caused by some human actor has an ideological background.
Equally, the counter-policy must be suggested by a party or
implemented by a government, which also has an ideological
background. Since ideology is a guideline for individuals to
evaluate specific situations or policies, it also influences citizens
preferences. Ideology can be described as an “interrelated set
of attitudes, values, and beliefs wit h cognitive, affective, and
motivational properties” (
Jost et al., 2009, p. 315). Ideology
groups peoples attitudes and experiences so they can be used
as guidelines for future decisions.
Ideology as a factor in policy preferences has been examined
for a long time (Stimson, 1975). First, the specific content
of policies can be more appealing to citizens with a certain
ideology. Second, a policy is proposed by a party, which can
be used as additional guidance by citizens. Research has shown
that partisans are more likely to support policies when they
are proposed by in-group partisan elites rather than out-group
partisan elites (
Bolsen et al., 2014; Pink et al., 2021). Intolerance
exists on both sides of t he ideological spectrum toward the
other side: “conservatism would predict intolerance of left-
wing targets, liberalism would predict intolerance of right-
wing targets. Moreover [...] t hose on both the left and right
would be biased against ideologically opposing targets relative to
ideologically supporting targets” (
Crawford and Pilanski, 2014,
p. 842). While partisanship is not exactly the same as ideology,
the two concepts are highly related and correlated (
Wright
et al., 1985
; Barber and Pope, 2019; Lupton et al., 2020). In
this paper, I will mainly refer to partisanship as an outcome of
ideology. Partisanship is heavily used by citizens to identify with
politicians and take their position on issues under discussion.
Policies concerning the nexus between security and civil
liberties are no exception when it comes to the impact of
citizens partisanship on their policy preferences. As these
policies are suggested by politicians, the match or mismatch
between citizens ideology and that of these politicians should be
a strong indicator of whether a citizen supports t he policy. On
average, when the policy-proposing party is known by citizens
the support for the policy decreases because there are always
citizens who dislike a given party. In contrast, when the citizens
are inclined to the policy-proposing party, the support for the
policy should increase.
H2a: Citizens suppor t for counter-terrorism measures
decreases when the policy-proposing party is disliked by them.
H2b: Citizens support for counter-terrorism measures
increases when they are inclined to the policy-proposing party.
Ideology is also inherent in the specific context I am
investigating—terrorism. Conveying an ideologically motivated
message is essential in many definitions of terrorism (
Ruby,
2002
; Schmid, 2011). While civil liberties have been widely
studied in the context of terrorism, terrorism has mostly
been considered as a general concept with no further
Frontiers in Political Science 04 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
specification. This introduces another factor in citizens support
for counter-terrorism policies, which is the possible alignment
of perpetrators motivation or ideology and the one of citizens
(
Caton and Mullinix, 2022). Ideology can serve as a guiding
factor, which allows citizens to allocate themselves to groups,
such as parties or interest groups. In an extreme case, citizens
could sympathize with potential terrorists and their motivations.
While most citizens are likely to condemn any type of
terrorism, supporters of extreme ideologies might not oppose
such acts as strongly as ot hers. In a polarized society, citizens can
be expected to support extreme versions of their own ideology.
While most people will still not directly support terrorism, they
might not support security measures that aim to prevent such
incidents as strongly as other citizens.
H2c: Citizens suppor t for counter-terrorism measures
decreases when they share the ideology of the terrorist actors.
The previous hypothesis concerned the influence of
partisanship and ideology on citizens’ attitudes toward counter-
terrorism measures. As outlined, the effect is expected to be
very strong and persistent. However, partisan loyalty will not be
unlimited. When citizens deviate from the lines of parties they
support is still debated. Related to this is the question of what
factors influence the formation of citizens policy preferences.
Can the dominant factor of party cues (
Cohen, 2003) be
overruled by other factors?
A likely case of deviation from the party lines is when
politicians behave undemocratically or propose policies that
contradict democratic norms. Studies conducted in t he U.S.,
however, have yielded contradictory findings.
Graham and
Svolik
(2020) find that in the U.S., partisanship is more
important to citizens than democratic norms: they would
rather stick to their ideologically close candidate who violates
democratic norms than vote for the opposition. Saikkonen
and Christensen
(2022) report similar findings for Finland.
Carey et al. (2022) come to the opposing conclusion that
citizens are willing to punish undemocratic behavior regardless
of partisanship. While the violation of democratic norms is an
extreme case, it can generally be expected that citizens rather
take t he position of their preferred party.
A most likely c ase of deviation from the party lines
occurs when the fundamentals of human life are in danger
or threatened. Security and the need for physical integrity
is at the very bottom of human necessities (
Maslow,
1954
). Since perceived threat has been identified as a
strong predictor for citizens need for security, perceived
threat should outweigh effects of partisanship. I expect
citizens to support a security policy even if they dislike t he
policy-proposing party.
H3a: Citizens suppor t for counter-terrorism measures
increases when they are personally threatened by an attack,
even when they dislike the policy-proposing party.
Another trade-off or contradiction can appear between party
preference and the suggested counter-terrorism policy. Citizens
could sympat hize with a party, but dislike t heir suggested policy.
More specifically, citizens could disagree with the policy-target,
especially if the target is their own in-group. In this case, citizens
are more likely to oppose a policy because they are constrained
by the policy. When polices are targeted toward an in-group,
partisanship should be overruled by the attitudes toward the
content of the policy (
Nicholson, 2012). Therefore, I expect that
support for a dragnet policy is slightly less likely than support for
a targeted or not specified policy. However, the level of citizens
support should still be comparatively high when the suggested
policy is proposed by their preferred party.
H3b: Citizens support for counter-terrorism measures
decreases when they are the target of that policy, even when
they are inclined to the policy-proposing party.
3. Data and method
3.1. Sample
I analyze data from a pre-registered
2
survey experiment
conducted in Germany in June 2022. The sample matches
the general population in terms of age, gender, and education
(N = 2,045).
3
No sampling weights were applied, since
highly qualitative survey data is giving precise estimates
while preserving high statistical power (
Miratrix et al.,
2018
). The experiment was part of a larger survey; the
median response time was 20.02 min. At the beginning of
the survey, an attention check was included; participants
who failed the attention check were excluded from further
participation in the survey and no answers were collected
from them (91.73 percent of the respondents passed t he
attention check).
3.2. Experimental setup
The study uses a 4 × 3 × 3 × 3 full factorial between-
subjects design (
Auspurg and Hinz, 2015) resulting in 108
unique vignettes in total, of which every respondent randomly
2 Pre-registration plan on osf: https://osf.io/7rs5v. Note that the
wording of hypothesis H1 and H3a has been changed from “targeted”
to “personally threatened” to make the wording consistent throughout
the study. In H2b, H3a, and H3b, “ideology” has been replaced with
“inclined/disinclined” to match the wording in the hypothesis with the
design of the experiment. The wording of H3b has been rearranged to
match the wording of H3a.
3 The study was conducted by the survey company Bilendi & respondi.
The distribution of demographic variables in the sample can be found in
the Appendix (
Supplementary material).
Frontiers in Political Science 05 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
received one. In a short text (Sauer et al., 2020), respondents
were asked to imagine a terror ist attack. The following paragraph
shows an example of a vignette with manipulated dimensions
highlighted in italics:
Imagine that a terrorist attack conducted by a right-wing
group takes place. An explosion occurs, injuring several
people. There is a serious danger for citizens like you, your
family and friends.
To ensure that attacks like this are prevented in the
future, politicians from The Greens want to increase
surveillance measures. These measures shall target
every citizen in the country. The measure includes the
monitoring of telephone c alls, letter mail, e-mails, and
social m edia accounts, as well as chats on cell phones
or smartphones.
Details of this attack are described using the four treatment
dimensions. Two dimensions each describe the terrorist attack
and the counter-terrorism measure. The first dimension
describes the motivation of the perpetrator as Islamist, right-
wing radical, or climate-radical. Islamist terrorism became
very prominent through the attacks of 9/11. Since it is well-
known and investigated very broadly, I included this attribute
to contextualize the other two motivations: right-wing and
climate-radical. Right-wing terrorism is a common source of
terrorism and has been present for over a decade in western
democracies. Especially in Germany, where the study was
fielded, right-wing motivated terrorism is the predominant form
and responsible for the largest attacks.
4
The third attribute
is climate-radical terrorism, often also called ecoterrorism.
Even though this type of terrorism is less well-known as
attacks with a different background, I argue that it is the
most promising motivation to test my hypothesis for several
reasons: First, it is not completely unknown, since ecoterrorism
as a term has appeared in mainstream media in the last two
decades, for example in the U.S. (
Smith, 2008). Some att acks of
radical environmentalist have been classified as terrorist attacks
(
Hirsch-Hoefler and Mudde, 2014).
5
Second, environmental
4 Recent examples are the shooting in Hanau in 2020 (https://www.
nytimes.com/2020/02/20/world/europe/germany-hanau-shisha-bar-
shooting.html
, accessed July 15, 2022) and the antisemitic attack in
Halle in 2019 (
https://www.theguardian.com/world/2019/oct/09/two-
people-killed-in-shooting-in-german-city-of-halle
, accessed July 15,
2022). In the early 2000s, a series of attacks was conducted by the
Nationalist Social Underground ( NSU). Reports in the media lasted for
several years due to a long trial (
https://www.nytimes.com/2013/05/
07/world/europe/trial-of-neo-nazi-beate-zschape-in-germany.html
,
accessed July 15, 2022).
5 For a historical overview, see,
Loadenthal, 2017. An exemplary
group is the Earth Liberation Front (ELF), w ho for example
attacked private property in the U.S. in 2008 (
https://web.
archive.org/web/20080306184703/http://ap.google.com/article/
ALeqM5hQlKz_UjBgvhm8rfGiTaQYS82a5gD8V66KUG0
, accessed
protection and its implication to slow down climate change are
very salient in the public discourse. In Germany, it was one
of the major issues during the last national election campaign
in 2021. Many people have a strong opinion on the issue.
It is not too hard to imagine that somebody with extreme
attitudes toward climate protection will turn to terrorism at
some point to underline their message with violence. There
are already books with activist intent that discuss the use
of violence to highlight the importance of mitigating climate
change, e.g.,
Malm, 2021. Third, this setup of right-wing
and climate-radical motivation mirrors the setup of the third
treatment dimension, the party that proposes the counter-
terrorism policy. For this dimension, I use the Alternative
für Deutschland (AfD) and the Bündnis 90/Die Grünen (The
Greens). While the AfD is classified as the furthest to the right
of the major parties in Germany, The Greens take strongly
opposing positions to the AfD on many policy issues. Therefore,
the parties are very distinguishable and appeal to different
people
6
.
The second treatment dimension describes whether
someone is personally threatened by a terrorist att ack (or not).
The fourth dimension describes whether someone is targeted
by a counter-terrorism policy (or not). Every dimension also
contains a control group, in which the attribute was not
specified or mentioned
7
.
3.3. Measures
The treatment text concludes with a description of a security
policy involving the surveillance of telephone calls, letters, e-
mails, social media accounts, and chats on cell phones or
smartphones. Afterwards, the respondents were asked to state
to which degree they would support the surveillance policy on a
ten-point scale. This serves as main dependent variable for the
study. As stated earlier, surveillance ent ails the conflict between
security and privacy r ights and can restrict ordinary citizens. The
case of surveillance is also well-suited t o examine the hypothesis
about partisanship, since the “cuts into pr ivacy rights beyond
what voters accept should reduce political support for the
incumbent” (
Epifanio, 2011, p. 403). Therefore, differently than
in the case of other counter-terrorism measures, the preferences
September 28, 2022) and a cableway in Germany in 2013 (https://
web.archive.org/web/20130905085530/http://www.ndr.de/regional/
niedersachsen/harz/seilbahn167.html
, accessed September 28, 2022).
6 The alternative would have been to use left-wing terrorism as
opposed to right-wing terrorism. However, left-wing terrorism has not
been present in the past two decades. Also, the political party in Germany
furthest to the left is quite unpopular, with a voter turnout of 5%.
7 See Appendix (
Supplementary material) for an overview of the
dimensions and attributes and detailed vignette wording.
Frontiers in
Political Science 06 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
for surveillance really be come a balancing act between security
and civil liberties.
To identify respondents’ partisanship, I use a pre-treatment
measure that asks respondents to rate their level of liking or
support for each party on a scale from one (“st rong aversion”) to
10 (“strong inclination”). The three lowest categories are coded
as aversion, the three highest as inclination and t he remaining
four as neutral
8
.
3.4. Analysis
To test the impact of the single dimensions, I compare
average marginal component effects (AMCEs;
Hainmueller
et al., 2014
).
9
To study the interdependence of the dimensions,
e.g., terrorist threat and being target of a policy, I use the
framework by
Egami and Imai (2019).
4. Results
4.1. Main results
Figure 1 shows the AMCEs for the four treatment
dimensions. The first two dimensions concern the side of the
terrorists. For the first dimension, terrorist threat, the effect
does not differ between the control group and the treatment
group. The additional information in the hypothetical scenario
that respondents are in danger or not does not influence their
support for the surveillance policy. The second dimension, the
specification of the motivation of terrorists, also has no influence
on respondents’ support for the counter-terrorism measure.
The third and fourth dimension describe the counter-
terrorism side. A specification of the policy-proposing party
leads to a significant and substantial decrease in policy support
(0.49 [CI: 0.81, 0.17] when proposed by The Greens and
0.91 [CI: 1.23, 0.59] when proposed by the AfD). When
the policy-proposing party is not specified, respondents might
think of their preferred party (or at least not about a party they
dislike), which leads to a stronger support of the policy. The
majority of the respondents feel aversion toward the two parties
specified in the treatment (40.52 percent toward The Greens,
72.36 percent toward the AfD) or are neutral toward them (37.32
percent toward The Greens, 15.86 percent toward the AfD).
Only a small share is inclined to each of the two parties (22.16
8 In robustness analyses, I vary the thresholds for categorizing
respondents as inclined or disinclined to a party. For further validation,
a secondary ite m is used, which asks respondents for their vote choice if
a general election were held next Sunday (Sonntagsfrage).
9 I also present marginal means in the Appendix
(
Supplementary material) to avoid the problem of having a fixed
reference category
(Leeper et al., 2020).
FIGURE 1
Average marginal component eect for the four treatment
dimensions. The dependent variable is the support for a
surveillance policy (10-point scale). The reference category for
each dimension is a control attribute in which the dimension
was not mentioned or specified. Lines around the point
estimates indicate 95% confidence intervals.
percent to The Greens, 11.77 percent to the AfD).
10
Accordingly,
on average respondents do not have a positive attitude toward
the two parties, which leads to a lower support for the policy.
The fourth and last dimension specifies whether the
proposed surveillance policy should be dragnet or targeted at
suspect individuals or groups. Respondents support for t he
dragnet policy is substantially and significantly lower (0.57
[CI: 0.89, 0.25]) than for the not specified policy (control).
Respondents support for the targeted policy is substantially and
significantly higher (0.66 [CI: 0.34, 0.98]) than for the control.
These results do not corroborate Hypothesis 1a, because the
direct threat of a terrorist attack does not change respondents
policy support.
11
In contrast, there is strong evidence for
Hypothesis 1b: respondents do not want to be personally
restricted by the given surveillance policy and are less likely to
support dragnet measures.
12
10 Regularly conducted surveys about Germans’ party preferences
(Sonntagsfrage) show similar numbers for the survey period.
11 For a discussion see Section 5.
12 This finding is further supported by a manipulation check, which
shows that respondents feel significantly and substantially more restricted
when the described surveillance measure is dragnet and not targeted (see
Supplementary Section 5).
Frontiers in Political Science 07 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
FIGURE 2
Predicted values for support for the dierent surveillance policies based on personal threat. Lines around the point estimates indicate 95 %
confidence inte rvals.
4.2. Interactions
Figure 2 shows the predicted policy support dependent
on personal threat and policy scope. In the absence of
personal threat, there is no significant difference between
support for a targeted measure and support for a dragnet
surveillance measure. In contrast, when citizens are personally
threatened, the support for a targeted measure is significantly
and substantially larger than support for a dragnet measure.
While personal threat increases the preference for a targeted
over a dragnet measure, the effect differs from the expectations
in Hypothesis 1c that support for surveillance would be
higher under personal threat, independent of the policy scope.
Therefore, there is no supporting evidence for Hypothesis 1c.
Figure 3 shows the predicted values for respondents’ policy
support based on the policy-proposing party and respondents
inclination or aversion to the AfD or The Greens.
13
When the
policy is proposed by the AfD, support for the policy differs very
strongly between respondents who are averse toward the AfD
and respondents who are neutral or pro toward the AfD, by an
average of roughly two points on a ten-point scale. However,
respondents who sympathize with the AfD are not more likely to
support the policy than neutral respondents. The panel for The
Greens shows a similar picture. Policy support is identic al on
average among neutral and inclined respondents. Respondents
who are averse to The Greens are on average 1.3 points less likely
to suppor t the policy than respondents who are sympathetic with
or neutral to The Greens.
This evidence supports Hypothesis 2a that respondents will
be less likely to support a counter-terrorism me asure when it
is pr oposed by a party they dislike. There is no support for
Hypothesis 2b that respondents will be more likely to support
13 See Appendix (Supplementary material) for marginal means and
results using the respondents’ hypothetical party vote.
such a measure when they are inclined to t he policy-proposing
party than when the policy-proposing is not specified.
To test Hypothesis 2c, it is necessary to compare whether
the respondents have the same motivation as the terrorists.
Using the previous setup, I investigate whether partisans of
the AfD change their policy pr eference when the terrorists’
motivation is right-wing extremist. I repeat this procedure for
policy preferences of partisans of The Greens when terrorists’
motivation is climate-radical. The results in
Figure 4 show
that the motivation of terrorists does not influence the policy
support of partisans who have a similar ideology in a less
extreme form. Since partisans of a single party are still a
very heterogeneous group, I investigated additional subgroups
based on their left–right self-assessment, their environmental
attitudes, and their level of extremism. None of t hese yield
any reduction in support for the counter-terrorism measure.
Furthermore, I investigated combinations of these attributes:
a high propensity to violence/high degree of extremism and
a right self-assessment or a strong personal commitment to
environmental protection (right extremists, far-right violent
extremists, climate extremists, climate-violent extremists). Since
the subgroups in this analysis are very small, the results should
be taken carefully, but again t here is no difference in support
of these subgroups. Respondents preferences for supporting a
counter-terrorism measure is not driven by the motivation of
the terrorists; consequently Hypothesis 2c is not confirmed.
For the remaining two hypotheses, 3a and 3b, we have to
look at three-way interactions. For Hypothesis 3a, I compare
how support for a policy proposed by a party to which
respondents are averse changes depending on whether the
terrorist attack poses a personal threat to the respondents
(
Figure 5). When the policy is proposed by The Greens, a small
(+0.48) but not significant increase in support can be seen.
When the policy is proposed by the AfD, the support deceases
in comparison to the control group. This effect is even smaller
Frontiers in Political Science 08 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
(0.25) and not significant. For Hypothesis 3b, I compare how
support for a policy proposed by a party to which respondents
are inclined changes depending on the policy being dragnet
(
Figure 6). For both parties, I find a very small (Greens 0.35;
AfD 0.48) and not significant effect. In sum, the evidence does
not support Hypothesis 3a and 3b.
5. Discussion
In t his paper, I investigated how citizens cope with threat,
restriction, and ideology (in the form of partisanship) and what
influence this has on their preferences for civil liberties or
security. The central findings of the study are that (a) citizens
who are averse to the party that proposes a security policy
are clearly less likely to support this policy and (b) citizens
are less likely to support a security policy if it is dragnet and
therefore restricts the citizens themselves. These two attributes
dominated the preference formation in comparison to other
factors. The study extends our understanding of (1) the influence
of political polarization in a context in which citizens prefer a
policy but have an aversion to the political actor that proposes
the policy, (2) citizens’ s upport for surveillance when they
are personally affected, and (3) the rejection of any kind of
terrorism, independent of the underlying ideology.
A direct translation to the real world depends on parties
behavior after an attack. If parties stand united, a rally-’round-
the-flag effect can occur, in which partisanship plays a minor
role and citizens support the governments action (
Kam and
Ramos, 2008
). However, parties do not necessarily stand united.
For example, in the aftermath of the aforementioned right-
wing terrorist attack on people believed to have a migrant
background in 2020 in Hanau, Germany, parties have not taken
a unified position. Medeiros and Makhashvili (2022) examined
the discourse on Twitter, where parties and individual politicians
expressed their condolence, but also stated their opinions.
The evolving discourse consisted of mainly two clusters. The
first cluster contained messages from journalists, legacy media
accounts, anti-racist activists, and politicians from the SPD and
Die Linke (center-left and left-wing party). The second cluster
was centered around the AfD, far-right political actors, and
far-right spam accounts. The authors interpret the discourse
as polarized (
Medeiros and Makhashvili, 2022, p. 45). This
example is not an exception of the AfD being isolated in their
position (
Urman, 2020). In such a case, i.e., when parties appear
not uniform, the results of the study are likely to hold. The
acceptance of policy proposals will depend to a certain degree
on the policy-proposing party.
While this study investigated the specific context of
terrorism, its implications are likely to apply to other external
shocks. Terrorism is comparable to the threat of violence from
other sources such as war or crime. Similarities appear in
two regards, in the physical dimension and in the symbolic
dimension (
Vergani, 2018, p. 23). This has also been shown in
empirical studies that compared citizens willingness to accept
cuts in civil liberties when they are faced with crime instead of
terrorism (
Mondak and Hurwitz, 2012).
The manipulation of threat as a single factor had no
influence on citizens support for surveillance (contrary to the
expectations stated in Hypothesis 1a). This is in line with
other experimental studies (such as Helbling et al., 2022),
in which a treatment that included terrorist threat did not
influence citizens policy attitudes. However, it contradicts other
experimental studies that have successfully shown an impact of
terrorist threat on policy support by mentioning the number of
victims in the past 3 years in their treatment (
Ziller and Helbling,
2021
). In the present study, citizens were asked to imagine a
terrorist attack. This attack was described as (a) threatening
for the individual citizen, their friends, and family, (b) not
threatening for the individual citizen, their friends, and family,
or (c) not further specified. Accordingly, respondents always had
to imagine a terrorist attack. Even though respondents in group
(c) did not get any information whether the threat was personal
or not, they still were asked to t hink about a terrorist attack.
Since terrorism is generally t hreatening, this could explain why
no differences were found between the threat conditions. An
alternative explanation would be a disconnection of perceived
threat and actual situations: “Subjectively perceived threats do
not necessarily have to correspond to an objectively threatening
situation—if the latter can be determined at all” (
Trüdinger,
2019
, p. 33). If citizens did not perceive the shown description
as personally threatening , but feel threatened by terrorism in
general, then we would expect no impact of the treatment
dimension. This argument is supported by an additional analysis
in which the perceived personal threat is investigated based
on the treatment dimensions. The manipulation of threat
did not change how respondents perceived personal threat
(see
Supplementary Section 5). A third explanation might be
that social threat instead of personal threat explains citizens
preferences to increase security at the cost of civil liberties
(
Huddy et al., 2002, p. 488). Counter-terrorism policies are
rather an answer to threat that affects the whole of society than
to one that affects the individual. As previous research on policy
preferences has shown, people try to evaluate what is not only
the best for themselves but also for their surroundings (
Sears
et al., 1980
). Therefore, they do not necessarily form policy
preferences only according to their own situation and feelings.
However, this explanation is less convincing since empirical
studies have already shown the impact of personal threat on
security preferences (Hetherington and Suhay, 2011; Asbrock
and Fritsche, 2013
).
For the impact of personal threat on support for targeted
and dragnet policies, the result was rather surprising (see
Figure 2). Personal threat does not increase the overall
support for counter-terrorism policies (as expected prior
to the study, stated in Hypothesis 1c), but instead changes
Frontiers in Political Science 09 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
FIGURE 3
Predicted values for support for the surveillance policy based on the policy-proposing party and the inclination/disinclination to the respective
party. Lines around the point estimates indicate 95% confidence intervals.
FIGURE 4
Predicted values for the support of the surveillance policy based on the motivation of terrorists and the inclination/disinclination to the AfD or
The Greens. Lines around the point estimates indicate 95% confidence intervals.
FIGURE 5
Predicted values for support of the surveillance policy. The policy is proposed by a party citizens are averse to. Results are shown for dierent
degrees of exposure to threat. Lines around the point estimates indicate 95% confidence intervals.
Frontiers in Political Science 10 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
FIGURE 6
Predicted values for support of the surveillance policy. The policy is proposed by a party citizens are inclined to. Results are shown for dierent
policy types. Lines around the point estimates indicate 95% confidence intervals.
the preference for the type of policy. When citizens are
under thr eat, the support for targeted measures increases
and the support for dragnet measures decreases compared
to measures when citizens are under no personal threat.
One possible explanation could be that people who are
personally threatened do not want to be additionally
targeted by a policy. They do not want to carry a double
burden. Testing this hypothesis or alternative ones is left to
future research.
In contrast to perceived threat, partisanship has a
strong influence on citizens’ support for a security policy.
However, real-world threat is expected to reduce the
strength of partisan cues. Instead of relying on party cues
only, citizens make use of the best evidence they can find
(
Druckman et al., 2021). In the case of the here discussed
security policy, citizens would be expected to evaluate how
effective the policy is to prevent them from threat. When
considered effective, the impact of partisanship is expected
to decrease. This relationship remains to be investigated in
future studies.
The second dimension in which no differences in policy
support was found concerns the motivation of the terrorists
in the treatment. Hypothesis 2c stated that the support
for counter-terrorism measures would decrease when citizens
share the ideology of terrorist actors. While it was quite
unlikely to find support for terrorist actions in the general
population, it is reassuring that terrorist motivation does not
influence citizens support for counter-measures. Normatively
speaking, this is positive news for democracy. A natural
experiment has shown that right-wing extremist attacks
shifted citizens who hold a rig ht ideology away from
this ideology (
Pickard et al., 2022). The present study
contributes to this finding by showing that other ideologies
also do not lead respondents to change their preference for
civil liberties.
Lastly, there was no significance for the small effect
sizes for Hypothesis 3a and 3b. Personal threat did not
overrule the disliking of a party and becoming target of
surveillance did not lower citizens policy support when the
policy was proposed by the citizens’ preferred party. Since
three-way interactions were needed in this study to test
these hypotheses, the experimental power was rather low,
which makes it difficult to detect small effect sizes. As a
result, these hypotheses cannot be rejected with hig h certainty.
Instead, this provides ground for future research with more
tailored experimental designs to examine the relationship
between action by preferred parties and restrictions for
individual citizens.
Subject to the limitations noted above, the findings
indicate that citizens attitudes toward security are rather
shaped by counter-terrorism than by terrorism. First, citizens
agreement with security policies rather depends on the
scope of the policy and whether they are affected by it.
Second, their support of these policies depends on their
liking or disliking of the policy-proposing party. Since
counter-terrorism, and the discussed issue of surveillance in
particular, is preventive in nature, these factors outweigh the
terrorist motivation and personal threat. The study contributes
to the understanding of citizens preferences for security
policies in a context in which the need for such policies
is emphasized. In a broader sense, this has implications for
political polarization because citizens are less likely to support
otherwise preferred policies if they are proposed by a party the
citizens disliked.
Frontiers in Political Science 11 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
Data availability statement
The dat a set and replication file are available online
(
https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/2ZOPHQ). Further inquiries can
be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed
and approved by Ethics Committee of the University of
Mannheim. The patients/participants provided their written
informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work
and has approved it for publication.
Funding
The publication of this article was funded by the Mannheim
Centre for European Social Research (MZES).
Conflict of interest
The aut hor declares that the research was conducted in
the absence of any commercial or financial relationships
that could be construed as a potential conflict
of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those
of the authors and do not necessarily represent those
of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher,
the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be
evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by
its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by t he
publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be
found online at:
https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/
fpos.2022.1006711/full#supplementary-material
References
Asbrock, F., and Fritsche, I. (2013). Authoritarian reactions to terrorist
threat: who is being threatened, the m e or the we? Int. J. Psychol. 48, 35–49.
doi: 10. 1080/00207594. 2012.69 5075
Auspurg, K., and Hinz, T. (2015). Factorial Survey Ex periments. Thousand Oaks,
CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. doi: 10.4135/9781483398075
Barber, M., and Pope, J. C. (2019). Does party Trump ideology?
Disentangling party and ideology in America. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 113, 38–54.
doi: 10. 1017/S0003055418000795
Bolsen, T., Druckman, J. N., and Cook, F. L. (2014). The influence of
Partisan motivated reasoning on public opinion. Polit. Behav. 36, 235–262.
doi: 10. 1007/s11109-013-9238- 0
Bozzoli, C., and Müller, C. (2011). Perceptions and attitudes following
a terrorist shock: evidence from the UK. Eur. J. Polit. Econ. 27, S89–S106.
doi: 10. 1016/j.ejpoleco.2011.06.005
Breznau, N. (2021). The welfare state and risk perceptions: The novel
coronavirus pandemic and public concern in 70 countries. Euro. Soc. 23, S33–S46.
doi: 10. 1080/14616696. 2020.17 93215
Carey, J., Clayton, K., Helmke, G., Nyhan, B., Sanders, M., and Stokes, S.
(2022). Who will defend democracy? Evaluating tradeoffs in candidate support
among partisan donors and voters. J. Elect. Public Opin. Parties. 32, 230–245.
doi: 10. 1080/17457289. 2020.17 90577
Caton, C., and Mullinix, K. J. (2022). Partisanship and support for restricting the
civil liberties of suspected terrorists. Polit. Behav. doi: 10.1007/s11109-022-09771-9
Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: the dominating impact of
group influence on political beliefs. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 808–822.
doi: 10. 1037/0022-3514. 85.5. 808
Cohen-Louck, K. (2019). Perception of the threat of terrorism. J. Interpers.
Violence 34, 887–911. doi: 10.1177/0886260516646091
Crawford, J. T., and Pilanski, J. M. (2014). Political intolerance, right and left.
Polit. Psychol. 35, 841–851. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2012.0092 6.x
Davis, D. W., and Silver, B. D. (2004). Civil liberties vs. security: public opinion
in the context of the terrorist attacks on America. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 48, 28–46.
doi: 10. 1111/j.0092-5853. 2004.0 0054.x
Druckman, J. N., Klar, S., Krupnikov, Y., Levendusky, M., and Ryan, J. B. (2021).
Affective polarization, local contexts and public opinion in America. Nat. Hum.
Behav. 5, 28–38. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-01012-5
Egami, N., and Imai, K. (2019). Causal interaction in factorial
experiments: application to conjoint analysis. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 114, 529–540.
doi: 10. 1080/01621459. 2018.14 76246
Epifanio, M. (201 1). Legislative response to international terrorism. J. Peace Res.
48, 3 99–411. doi: 10.1177/0022343311399130
Feldman, S. (2013). Comments on: authoritarianism in social context: the role
of threat. Int. J. Psychol. 48, 55–59. doi: 10. 1080/00207594.20 12.742196
Fritsche, I., Jonas, E., and Kessler, T. (2011). Collective reactions to thre at:
implications for intergroup conflict and for solving societal crises. Soc. Issues Policy
Rev. 5, 101–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2011.01027.x
Gadarian, S. K. (2010). The politics of threat: how terrorism news shapes foreign
policy attitudes. J. Polit. 72, 469–483. doi: 10.1017/S0022381609990910
Giani, M., Epifanio, M., and Ivandic, R. (2021). Wait and see? Public opinion
dynamics after terrorist attacks. socarXiv. doi: 10.31235/osf.io/qt7s4
Graham, M. H., and Svolik, M. W. (2020). Democracy in America? Partisanship,
polarization, and the robustness of support for democracy in the United States. Am.
Polit. Sci. Rev. 114, 392–4 09. doi: 10.1017/S0003055420000052
Haider-Markel, D. P., Joslyn, M. R., and Al-Baghal, M. T. (2006). Can
we frame the terrorist threat? Issue frames, the perception of threat, and
opinions on counterterrorism policies. Terror. Polit. Violence 18, 545–559.
doi: 10. 1080/0954655060088 0625
Hainmueller, J., Hopkins, D. J., and Yamamoto, T. (2014). Causal inference in
conjoint analysis: understanding multidimensional choices via stated preference
experiments. Polit. Anal. 22, 1–30. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpt024
Frontiers in Political Science 12 frontiersin.org
Jäger 10.3389/fpos.2022.1006711
Harteveld, E., and Wagner, M. (2022). Does affective polarisation increase
turnout? Evidence from Germany, the Netherlands and Spain. West Eur. Polit.
1–28. doi: 10.1080/01402382.2022.2087395
Hegemann, H., and Kahl, M. (2018). “Sicherheit vs. freiheit?
Probleme und dilemmata der terrorismusbekämpfung, in Terrorismus
und Terrorismusbekämpfung: Eine Einführung, Elemente der Politik, H.
Hegemann and M. Kahl (Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien), 169–188.
doi: 10. 1007/978-3-658 -16086-9_7
Helbling, M., and Meierrieks, D. (2020). Transnational terrorism and restrictive
immigration polici es. J. Peace Res. 57, 564–580. doi: 10.1177/0022343319897105
Helbling, M., Meierrieks, D., and Pardos-Prado, S. (2022). Terrorism
and immigration policy preferences. Defence Peace Econ. 1–14.
doi: 10. 1080/10242694. 2022.20 61837
Heltzel, G., and Laurin, K. (2020). Polarization in America: two possible futures.
Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 34, 179–184. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.03.008
Hetherington, M., and Suhay, E. (2011). Authoritarianism, threat, and
Americans support for the war on terror. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 55, 546–560.
doi: 10. 1111/j.1540-5907. 2011. 00514.x
Hirsch-Hoefler, S., and Mudde, C. (2014). “Ecoterrorism”:
terrorist threat or political ploy? Stud. Conflict Terror. 37, 586–603.
doi: 10. 1080/1057610X. 2014. 913121
Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Capelos, T., and Provost, C. (2002). The consequences of
terrorism: disentangling the effects of personal and national t h reat. Polit. Psychol.
23, 48 5–509. doi: 10.1111/0162-895X.00295
Huddy, L., Feldman, S., Taber, C., and Lahav, G. (2005). Threat,
anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 49, 593–608.
doi: 10. 1111/j.1540-5907. 2005. 00144.x
Huddy, L., Feldman, S., and Weber, C. (2007). The political consequences of
perceived threat and felt insecurity. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit . Soc. Sci. 614, 131–153.
doi: 10. 1177/000271620730 5951
Jenkins-Smith, H. C., and Herron, K. G. (2009). Rock and a hard place: public
willingness to trade civil rights and liberties for greater security. Polit. Policy 37,
1095–1129. doi: 10.1111/j.1747-1346.2009.00215.x
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., and Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: its
structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 307–337.
doi: 10. 1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163600
Jungkunz, S. (2021). Political polarization during the COVID-19 pandemic.
Front. Polit. Sci. 3, 622512. doi: 10.3389/fpos.2021.622512
Kam, C. D., and Ramos, J. M. (2008). Joining and leaving the rally:
understanding the surge and decline in presidential approval following 9/11. Public
Opin. Q. 72, 619–650. doi: 10.1093/poq/nfn055
Kawecki, D. (2022). End of consensus? Ideology, partisan identity and affective
polarization in Finland 2003–2019. socarXiv. doi: 10.31235/osf.io/4k7u9
Kossowska, M., Trejtowicz, M., de Lemus, S., Bukowski, M., Hiel, A. V., and
Goodwin, R. (2011). Relationships between right-wing authoritarianism, terrorism
threat, and attitudes towards restrictions of civil rights: A comparison among four
European countries. Brit. J. Psychol. 102, 245–259. doi: 10.1348/000712610X517262
Leeper, T. J., Hobolt, S. B., and Tilley, J. (2020). Measuring subgroup preferences
in c onjoint experiments. Polit. Anal. 28, 207–221. doi: 10.1017/pan.2019.30
Loadenthal, M. (2017). “Eco-terrorism”: an incident-driven history of attack
(1973–2010). J. Study Radical. 11, 1–34. doi: 10.14321/jstudradi.11.2.0001
Lupton, R. N., Smallpage, S. M., and Enders, A. M. (2020). Values and political
predispositions in the age of polarization: examining the relationship between
partisanship and ideology in the United States, 1988–2012. Brit. J. Polit. Sci. 50,
241–260. doi: 10.1017/S0007123417000370
MacKuen, M., and Brown, C. (1987). Political context and attitude change. Am.
Polit. Sci. Rev. 81, 471–490 . doi: 10.2307/1961962
Malm, A. (2021). How to Blow Up a Pipeline. Verso Books.
Marcus, G. E., Sullivan, J. L., Theiss-Morse, E., and Wood, S. L. (1995). With
Malice toward Some: How People Make Civil Liberties Judgments. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139174046
Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and Personality. New York, NY: Prabhat
Prakashan.
Medeiros, D., and Makhashvili, A. (2022). United in grief? Emotional
communities around the far-right terrorist attack in Hanau. Media Commun. 10,
39–49. doi: 10.17645/mac.v10i3.5438
Miratrix, L. W., Sekhon, J. S., Theodoridis, A. G., and Campos, L. F. (2018).
Worth weighting? How to think about and use weights in survey experiments.
Polit. Anal. 26, 275–291. doi: 10.1017/pan.2018.1
Mondak, J. J., and Hurwitz, J. (2012). Examin ing the terror exception:
terrorism and commitments to civil liberties. Public Opin. Q. 76, 193–213.
doi: 10. 1093/poq/nfr068
Mu noz, J., Falcó-Gimeno, A., and Hernández, E. (2020). Unexpected event
during survey design: promise and pitfalls for causal inference. Polit. Anal. 28,
186–206. doi: 10.1017/pan.2019.27
Nicholson, S. P. (2012). Polarizing cues. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 56, 52–66.
doi: 10. 1111/j.1540-5907. 2011.0 0541.x
Peffley, M., Knigge, P., and Hurwit z, J. (2001). A multiple values model of
political tolerance. Polit . Res. Q. 54, 379–406. doi: 10.1177/106591290105400207
Pickard, H., Efthyvoulou, G., and Bove, V. (2022). What’s left after
right-wing extremism? The effects on political orientation. Eur. J. Polit. Res.
doi: 10. 1111/1475-6765. 12538
Pink, S. L., Chu, J., Druckman, J. N., Rand, D. G., and Willer, R. (2021). Elite
party cues increase vaccination intentions among Republicans. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Sci. U.S.A. 118, e2106559118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2106559118
Pronin, E., Kennedy, K., and Butsch, S. (2006). Bombing versus negotiating: how
preferences for combating terrorism are affected by perceived terrorist rationality.
Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 385–392. doi: 10.1207/s15324834basp2804_12
Rohde, I. M. T., and Rohde, K. I. M. (2011). Risk attitudes in a social context. J.
Risk Uncertainty 43, 205–225. doi: 10.1007/s11166-011-9127-z
Ruby, C. L. (2002). The definition of terrorism. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 2,
9–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2002.00021. x
Saikkonen, I. A.-L., and Christensen, H. S. (2022). Guardians of democracy or
passive bystanders? A conjoint experiment on elite transgressions of democratic
norms. Polit. Res. Q. doi: 10.31235/osf.io/j6tvy
Sauer, C., Auspurg, K., and Hinz, T. (2020). Designing multi-factorial
survey experiments: effects of presentation style (text or table), answering scales,
and vignette order. methods data analyses 14, 195–214. doi: 10.12758/mda.
2020.06
Schmid, A. P. (2011). The Routledge Handbook of Terrorism Research. Abington;
New York, NY: Taylor & Francis. doi: 10.4324/9780203828731
Sears, D. O., Lau, R. R., Tyler, T. R., and Allen, H. M. (1980). Self-interest vs.
symbolic politics in policy attitudes and presidential voting. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 74,
670–684. doi: 10.2307/1958149
Siri, J. (2018). The alternative for Germany after the 2017 Election. German Polit.
27, 1 41–145. doi: 10.1080/09644008.2018.1445724
Smith, R. K. (2008). “Ecoterrorism”?: A critical analysis of the
vilification of radical environmental activists as terrorists. Environ. Law
38, 4 0.
Somer, M., and McCoy, J. (2018). Déjá vu? polarization and
endangered democracies in the 21st century. Am. Behav. Sci. 62, 3–15.
doi: 10. 1177/0002764218760 371
Stimson, J. A. (1975). Belief systems: constraint, complexity,
and the 1972 election. Am. J. Polit. Sci. 19, 393–417. doi: 10.2307/
2110536
Sullivan, J. L., and Hendriks, H. (2009). Public support for civil
liberties pre- and post-9/11. Annu. Rev. Law Soc. Sci. 5, 375–391.
doi: 10. 1146/annurev.lawsocsci.093008.131525
Svolik, M. W. (2019). Polarization versus democracy. J. Democracy 30, 20–32.
doi: 10. 1353/jod.2019.0039
Trüdinger, E.-M. (2019). Perceptions of threat and policy attitudes: The case of
support for anti-terrorism policies in Germany. Habilitation.
Urman, A. (2020). Context matters: political polarization on
Twitter from a comparative perspective. Media Cult. Soc. 42, 857–879.
doi: 10. 1177/0163443719876 541
van Leeuwen, M. (2003). “Democracy versus terrorism: balancing security and
fundamental rights, in Confronting Terrorism, ed M. van Leeuwen (Leiden: Brill |
Nijhoff), 227–233. doi: 10.1163/9789047403227_014
Vergani, M. (2018). How Is Terrorism Changing Us? Singapore: Springer
Singapore. doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-8066-1
Wright, G. C., Erikson, R. S., and McIver, J. P. (1985). Measuring
state partisanship and ideology with survey data. J. Polit. 47, 469–489.
doi: 10. 2307/2130892
Wynter, T. (2017). Counterterrorism, counterframing, and perceptions of
terrorist (i r)rationality. J. Glob. Sec. Stud. 2, 364–376. doi: 10.1093/jogss/ogx017
Ziller, C., and Helbling, M. (2021). Public support for state surveillance. Euro. J.
Polit. Res. 60, 994–1006. doi: 10.1111/1475-6765.12424
Frontiers in Political Science 13 frontiersin.org