This article was downloaded by: [108.179.189.54] On: 25 October 2013, At: 09:49
Publisher: Institute for Operations Research and the Management Sciences (INFORMS)
INFORMS is located in Maryland, USA
Organization Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://pubsonline.informs.org
Expressed Humility in Organizations: Implications for
Performance, Teams, and Leadership
Bradley P. Owens, Michael D. Johnson, Terence R. Mitchell
To cite this article:
Bradley P. Owens, Michael D. Johnson, Terence R. Mitchell (2013) Expressed Humility in Organizations: Implications for
Performance, Teams, and Leadership. Organization Science 24(5):1517-1538. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1120.0795
Full terms and conditions of use: http://pubsonline.informs.org/page/terms-and-conditions
This article may be used only for the purposes of research, teaching, and/or private study. Commercial use
or systematic downloading (by robots or other automatic processes) is prohibited without explicit Publisher
approval. For more information, contact [email protected].
The Publisher does not warrant or guarantee the article’s accuracy, completeness, merchantability, fitness
for a particular purpose, or non-infringement. Descriptions of, or references to, products or publications, or
inclusion of an advertisement in this article, neither constitutes nor implies a guarantee, endorsement, or
support of claims made of that product, publication, or service.
Copyright © 2013, INFORMS
Please scroll down for article—it is on subsequent pages
INFORMS is the largest professional society in the world for professionals in the fields of operations research, management
science, and analytics.
For more information on INFORMS, its publications, membership, or meetings visit http://www.informs.org
Organization Science
Vol. 24, No. 5, September–October 2013, pp. 1517–1538
ISSN 1047-7039 (print) ISSN 1526-5455 (online)
http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1120.0795
© 2013 INFORMS
Expressed Humility in Organizations:
Implications for Performance, Teams, and Leadership
Bradley P. Owens
The State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, New York 14260, bpowens@buffalo.edu
Michael D. Johnson, Terence R. Mitchell
University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195
{mdj3@uw.edu, trm@uw.edu}
W
e draw on eight different lab and field samples to delineate the effects of expressed humility on several important
organizational outcomes, including performance, satisfaction, learning goal orientation, engagement, and turnover.
We first review several literatures to define the construct of expressed humility, discuss its implications in social interactions,
and distinguish expressed humility from related constructs. Using five different samples, Study 1 develops and validates
an observer-report measure of expressed humility. Study 2 examines the strength of expressed humility predictions of
individual performance and contextual performance (i.e., quality of team member contribution) relative to conscientiousness,
global self-efficacy, and general mental ability. This study also reveals that with regard to individual performance, expressed
humility may compensate for lower general mental ability. Study 3 reports insights from a large field sample that examines
the relationship between leader-expressed humility and employee retention as mediated by job satisfaction and employee
engagement as mediated by team learning orientation. We conclude with recommendations for future research.
Key words: humility; task performance; leadership; turnover; self-awareness; narcissism; general mental ability;
learning goal orientation; construct validation; theory elaboration
History: Published online in Articles in Advance February 12, 2013.
Introduction
The construct of humility has received increasing atten-
tion in organizational scholarship in recent years. In the
wake of recent corporate scandals that have been
attributed to the unbridled ego, hubris, sense of entitle-
ment, and self-importance of the corporate executives
involved (Boje et al. 2004), virtues such as humility have
been viewed with greater interest and are seen as more
essential to the character of those who lead and work
within organizations. Humility has been included as one
of the core “organizational virtues” that are proposed to
provide the moral foundation of organizational environ-
ments (Cameron et al. 2003). Theorists have proposed
that humility is becoming more critical for leaders who
direct their organizations in increasingly dynamic and
turbulent environments (Morris et al. 2005, Vera and
Rodriguez-Lopez 2004). Weick (2001, p. 93) suggested,
for instance, that the increasing “unpredictability and
unknowability” organizations face will require leaders of
the 21st century to have “more humility and less hubris.
Despite calls for leaders and organization members to
have more humility, there are large gaps in our under-
standing of what “humility” means. Some view humil-
ity as a multifaceted, adaptive strength (Tangney 2000),
whereas others associate humility with humiliation and
lowliness (see study results of Exline and Geyer 2004).
Many definitions have been suggested, but little con-
ceptual consensus has emerged. Moreover, no empirical
research that we are aware of has measured the impact
of humility on important organizational outcomes. Con-
sequently, there are many propositions suggesting the
importance of humility within organizations (i.e., Collins
2005, Morris et al. 2005, Owens and Hekman 2012,
Vera and Rodriguez-Lopez 2004), but almost nothing
has been confirmed by empirical study. Given these mea-
surement problems and the interpersonal, interactional
nature of organizational life, we focus our definition on
the expressed behaviors that demonstrate humility and
how the behaviors are perceived by others.
To further the study of expressed humility in organi-
zational contexts we have three major goals. First, we
seek to define the construct in a way that integrates
previous work but goes beyond in a variety of ways
by (1) systematically synthesizing the existing litera-
ture into a parsimonious definition of expressed humil-
ity, (2) conceptually fitting expressed humility within
the nomological network of related constructs, and
(3) emphasizing how the expressed aspects of humil-
ity could influence important organizational behaviors.
Our second goal is to develop a measure of expressed
humility by following the standard procedures suggested
by Hinkin (1995). Our third goal is to demonstrate the
1517
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1518 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
usefulness of expressed humility, especially as expressed
by a leader in the organizational context.
Defining Expressed Humility
The general construct of humility has a rich back-
ground in theology and philosophy. Because humility
often entails the recognition and appreciation of knowl-
edge and guidance beyond the self, it is a founda-
tional principle in all major world religions—Buddhism,
Christianity, Hinduism, Islam, etc. Humility is also
central to many philosophical discussions of moral-
ity. Immanuel Kant, for example, viewed humility as
a “meta-attitude which constitutes the moral agent’s
proper perspective on himself and a virtue foundational
to most other virtues (Grenberg 2005, p. 133). Humility
in general has been categorized as a temperance virtue
that guards against excess (Park and Peterson 2003).
Although the virtue of humility has rich historical
roots, conceptualizations of humility vary significantly
across philosophical, theological, and psychological per-
spectives. These differing conceptualizations have made
it difficult to derive consensus about the definition of
humility. Furthermore, attempts to assimilate different
conceptualizations of humility from these fields has
led to complex definitions that, in some cases, con-
tain up to 13 different dimensions (Vera and Rodriguez-
Lopez 2004), making humility an unwieldy construct to
operationalize.
One possible reason why previous definitions of
humility are so complex is that humility may have
different forms or types. Some components of past
humility definitions have a clear intrapersonal, internal,
and personal focus (i.e., balanced processing of per-
sonal strengths and weaknesses; see Owens 2011); other
definitions capture the manifested, external, expressed,
or interpersonal properties of humility (i.e., display-
ing a penchant to seek to learn from others; see Vera
and Rodriguez-Lopez 2004). Given the conceptual and
operational implications for these differences, it is not
surprising that past definitions of humility have been
confusing and past attempts to measure humility unsuc-
cessful. Because our purpose is to examine humility in
the context of organizations, which is considered to gen-
erally be a rich social context, we focus our attention on
the expressed, interpersonal nature of humility.
Literature Review and Definition
A review of the current literature was our first step
in developing a definition of expressed humility. We
used three criteria in selecting articles for this analy-
sis. Specifically, we included articles (1) written within
the last 10 years, (2) whose main intent was to define
the humility construct, and (3) from psychological or
organizational behavior journals or book chapters. Given
our interest in the interpersonal and behavioral aspects
of humility, we looked specifically for examples of
humility that would be externally observable by oth-
ers (i.e., behavioral based) and did not consider more
intrapersonal aspects of humility (i.e., cognitions and
emotions). The first author and two other academic
researchers carefully reviewed this literature indepen-
dently and made notes on what emerged as the core
dimensions of expressed humility. We then met to dis-
cuss the components of humility and to present our sum-
maries of the components of expressed humility. After
some discussion, we agreed on three general categories
of behavior that underlie the construct. We converged
on three dimensions because with more categories, there
was redundancy across categories, and with fewer cat-
egories, some key aspects of expressed humility were
obscured.
As a result of our independent analysis of the litera-
ture and the previous definitions of humility (i.e., Morris
et al. 2005, Exline et al. 2004, Tangney 2002, Templeton
1997, Means et al. 1990), we define expressed humility
as an interpersonal characteristic that emerges in social
contexts that connotes (a) a manifested willingness to
view oneself accurately, (b) a displayed appreciation of
others’ strengths and contributions, and (c) teachability.
We view these components as the core reflective indica-
tors of humility (Bollen and Lennox 1991) and see them
as tightly interrelated. Expressed humility comprises a
pattern of behaviors that occur in interpersonal interac-
tions and is therefore observable by others. We propose
that though individuals may have a baseline penchant
for expressing humility that is based on heredity, social-
ization, and life experience, the expression of a person’s
humility may vary according to contextual cues and cir-
cumstances (Mischel and Shoda 1995). We elaborate on
each component of our definition of expressed humil-
ity below. Though our construct focuses on external,
observable behaviors that can be perceived by others,
we discuss general insights from the humility literature
about the motives and cognitions that are thought to
undergird the behavioral manifestations of humility.
An Interpersonal Characteristic
Similar to Costa and McCrae’s (1992) conceptualization
of extraversion and agreeableness as personal, individual
dispositions that are manifest on the “interpersonal
plane, expressed humility represents an individual
characteristic that emerges in social interactions, is
behavior based, and is recognizable to others. For
decades, the term “interpersonal traits” has been used
to conceptualize individual differences that shape inter-
personal interactions (Sullivan 1953, Bales 1950, Foa
and Foa 1974, Wiggins 1979) and have been effectively
assessed through peer ratings (Costa and McCrae 1992).
Though all personality traits have some interpersonal
implications—and indeed, the Big Five was originally
based on an analysis of social (peer-reported) ratings
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1519
(McCrae and Costa 1989)—some individual differences
implicate interpersonal dynamics more than others.
Like extraversion and agreeableness, we conceptualize
expressed humility as a construct that has significant
interpersonal implications. Because humility is usually a
label attached by others—not something that an individ-
ual assesses and asserts as part of his or her own self-
view—we view conceptualizing and operationalizing an
expressed or social form of humility as a meaningful and
effective way to begin studying this historically elusive
construct. We differentiate our social conceptualization
of humility from the more intrapersonal, philosophical
approaches to humility (e.g., self-transcendence, appre-
ciation of how all things are connected and contribute to
our cohesive universe; see Tangney 2002, Roberts and
Wood 2003), which have more intracognitive implica-
tions. Our social view of humility matches our purpose
to help us further understand how humility plays out in
organizational contexts.
We propose that expressed humility reflects a per-
son’s tendency to approach interpersonal interactions
with a strong motive for learning through others. Inter-
action “pushes us to constantly collect information about
ourselves and our environment” (Lawrence and Nohria
2002, p. 107), and therefore expressed humility is
primed, catalyzed, and reinforced through interactions
with others. In addition, humble people utilize interac-
tional partners as “social mirrors” through which they
can more accurately see (or gain an accurate reflec-
tion of) themselves by being transparent about strengths
and limitations. And finally, humble people see inter-
action partners as key sources of learning and insights
about personal development. Given its relational nature,
expressed humility will be especially relevant in con-
texts that entail frequent interactions with people and
where the content of the interactions is to exchange
information, feedback, and criticism (such as in leader
follower dyads).
Manifested Willingness to See the Self Accurately
The first component of our definition of expressed
humility captures a desire to engage in an ongoing pro-
cess of achieving accurate self-awareness through inter-
actions with others. Nielsen et al. (2010) suggested that
“people with humility are actively engaged in utilizing
information gathered in interactions with others, not only
to make sense of, but also, when necessary, to mod-
ify the self. That is, their self-views are focused on
their interdependence with others rather than their inde-
pendence from others” (pp. 34–35). We propose that
humility fosters a more objective appraisal of personal
strengths and limitations that is manifested by transpar-
ent disclosure of personal limits, acknowledging mis-
takes, and seeking realistic feedback about the self. From
a general psychological standpoint, longitudinal research
has shown that individuals who maintain more realistic
self-views tend to be more psychologically healthy and
have higher general well-being (Vaillant 1992). In con-
trast, individuals with self-enhancing and inaccurately
positive self-perceptions (which are often measured by
contrasting self-views with those of observers) are more
likely to be maladjusted over the long term (Ungerer
et al. 1997), be more deceitful, have a more brittle ego-
defense system, have less social poise and presence, and
be less productive (Colvin et al. 1995).
In an organizational context, these behaviors imply
that this component of the expressed humility construct
may have implications for the quality of interpersonal
work relationships, decision making, and subsequent
performance. We propose that employees who engage
in this self-learning through interactions with others
gain a more accurate awareness of their own intraper-
sonal resources and will be less prone to overconfidence,
which is the root of a myriad of organizational prob-
lems, poor decisions, and self-complacency (McIntyre
and Salas 1995, Vera and Rodriguez-Lopez 2004).
Obviously, this overconfidence is especially danger-
ous in those who hold power in organizations, such as
those in leadership roles. A balanced or more accurate
self-awareness helps organizational members and lead-
ers more accurately know when to take action and when
to learn more about an issue, as well as which endeav-
ors they are capable of handling competently and which
should not be attempted because of incompetence or
inexperience. Furthermore, as past research has shown
that self-disclosure often leads to increased trust, rela-
tional satisfaction, and reciprocal disclosure (Ehrlich and
Graeven 1971, Collins and Miller 1994), we believe
that this dimension of expressed humility, manifested by
disclosure of personal limitations and weaknesses, has
the potential to foster higher-quality and more transpar-
ent interpersonal interactions between leaders, followers,
and peers.
Appreciation of Others’ Strengths and
Contributions
According to Means et al. (1990, p. 214), “Humility
is an increase in the valuation of others and not a
decrease in the valuation of self. Expressed humility
reflects attitudes that are other-enhancing rather than
self-enhancing (Morris et al. 2005) and leads one to
acknowledge and show that he values others’ strengths
(Tangney 2002). Humility allows one to transcend the
comparative–competitive response when interacting with
others and instead acknowledge and admire the strengths
and contributions of others without feeling threatened
by them (Exline et al. 2004). In line with this reason-
ing, King and Hicks (2007, p. 632) suggested that “true
humility is more about possessing an exalted view of
the capacities of others rather than a negative view of
oneself. Expressed humility entails the behavioral man-
ifestation of this positive view of others.
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1520 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
From an organizational perspective, research has
found that when individuals are given power they tend
to devalue the worth and contributions of others (Kipnis
1972). We propose that organizational leaders and mem-
bers who possess humility can transcend this tendency
and instead maintain a genuine appreciation and high
valuation of the efforts, strengths, and abilities of their
coworkers. This is not to say that individuals with humil-
ity will be oblivious to their coworkers’ weaknesses and
areas of incompetence, but rather, they will be able to
more readily identify and value the unique abilities and
strengths of those with whom they work. By attending to
the qualities of others, humble organizational members
will more likely hold a nonheuristical, complex view of
others (i.e., viewing others through a multifaceted lens
that sees a variety of character strengths and skill sets
others possess) and less likely to hold simplistic, dualis-
tic evaluations of others (i.e., competent versus incom-
petent). By so doing, humble leaders and members are
more readily able to identify in others valuable resources
for social modeling and learning.
Teachability
The third main component of expressed humility is
teachability, which is manifested by showing open-
ness to learning, feedback, and new ideas from others.
Tangney (2000, p. 72) argued that “humility carries with
it an open-mindedness, a willingness to 0 0 0 seek advice,
and a desire to learn. Similarly, Templeton (1997,
p. 162) noted, “Inherent in humility resides an open and
receptive mind 0 0 0 it leaves us more open to learn from
others. We propose that this aspect of expressed humil-
ity reflects a person’s absorptive capacity (see Zahra
and George 2002) on an individual level and may be
generally related to developmental readiness, a concept
that has mainly been applied to leaders (Avolio et al.
2009) within the context of interpersonal interactions.
This aspect of expressed humility would be manifest by
a displayed receptiveness to others’ feedback, ideas, and
advice and the willingness to ask for help.
The ability of organizational members to learn effec-
tively is critical for organizations competing in the
present “knowledge economy” (Dane and Pratt 2007).
The rapid advance of technology and increasing special-
ization of work suggest that organizations are in greater
need of leaders and employees who are teachable and
have a desire and willingness to acquire new skills,
absorb new information, and learn from others. Hum-
ble individuals, through showing teachability, afford oth-
ers a sense of voice, which has been shown to foster
greater trust, motivation, and a heightened sense of jus-
tice (Cropanzano et al. 2007). Teachability may also be a
particularly important component of expressed humility
in leadership contexts. Alexander and Wilson (in Church
et al. 1998) argued that a thirst for learning is one of the
most critical capacities of effective leaders. Tichy and
Cohen (1997) also insisted on the importance of leaders
having a “teachable point of view.” Finally, Weick (2001,
p. 110) stated that when a leader is able to humbly admit,
“I don’t know, “that admission forces the leader to drop
pretense, drop omniscience, drop expert authority, drop
a macho posture, and drop monologues 0 0 0 0 [L]istening
and exploring is the consequence.
Expressed Humility in the Nomological Network
In the exploration of a new construct, it is important
to explain where it fits within the nomological network
and its unique conceptual space (Schwab 1980, Hinkin
1995). Expressed humility is related to but distinct from
many existing constructs. From our review of the litera-
ture, it seems most important to differentiate expressed
humility from the existing constructs of modesty, narcis-
sism, openness to experience, honesty-humility, learning
goal orientation (LGO), and core self-evaluation (CSE).
1
Table 1 summarizes what we view as the conceptual dif-
ferences between these constructs and the dimensions of
expressed humility.
Although we do not need to go over each of the vari-
ables in the table, we summarize by saying that in every
comparison with expressed humility, the other constructs
demonstrate either criterion deficiencies or criterion con-
tamination. That is, the related constructs either fail to
capture the complete expressed humility construct space
or contain aspects of their construct that are not part
of the expressed humility space, and in some instances,
both problems occur. For instance, that modesty does not
include a learning orientation, antinarcissism does not
capture teachability, and openness does not include self-
evaluative information are all examples of criterion defi-
ciency. Criterion contamination examples would include
learning goal orientation targeting achievement situa-
tions and core self-evaluations having optimistic self-
views instead of accurate ones.
Measuring Expressed Humility
As previously mentioned, most of the work discussing
humility in organizations has been theoretical, and thus,
to test these theories, a reliable and valid measure of
humility must be developed. In line with the concept
of expressed humility, we developed a measure that
reflects the perceptions of humble behavior expressed
by a focal person. Here is why: First, initial attempts
to measure humility have questionable face and con-
struct validity. Past attempts to circumvent problems
associated with measuring humility (e.g., having people
accurately self-report their strengths and weaknesses or
describing themselves as humble) have used a variety
of proxies aimed at indirectly representing the humil-
ity construct. Some have tried to avoid the problems
inherent in measuring humility via self-report by sim-
ply operationalizing humility as low self-esteem (Knight
and Nadel 1986). Others have discussed the possibil-
ity of measuring humility by comparing the differ-
ence between self-report and other-report performance
evaluations (Tangney 2000).
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1521
Table 1 Expressed Humility in the Nomological Network
Constructs Definition Key differences from expressed humility
Modesty Not boastful, underselling accomplishments,
lacking assertiveness. Withhold positive
information about the self (Baumeister and
Jones 1978); deferring credit for success
(Hareli and Weiner 2000).
Modesty differs from expressed humility in that modesty
has less to do with the motivation for personal learning
and personal development and more to do with having
the social sensitivity not to draw too much attention to,
talk too much about, or boast about oneself.
Narcissism “A personality trait encompassing grandiosity,
arrogance, self-absorption, entitlement,
fragile self-esteem, and hostility” (Rosenthal
and Pittinsky 2006, p. 617).
Expressed humility is not merely a lack of displayed
grandiosity or self-absorption (as we would expect from
low levels of narcissism), but it also addresses
teachability or appreciation of others, dimensions that are
not directly addressed by narcissism. Expressed humility
also is theorized to be associated with a stable or
tempered self-view that does not overinflate with praise
or overdeflate with criticism; narcissism refers to stark
oscillations from grandiose to dejected self-views.
Openness to experience Describes the extent to which someone is
imaginative and insightful; has a broad
range of interests; is in tune with inner
feelings; and appreciates art, emotion,
novelty, unusual ideas, and adventure
(Barrick and Mount 1991, McCrae and
Costa 1987).
Expressed humility focuses on being open to feedback and
ideas from others (and thus is more of an interpersonal
construct) and captures one’s approach to self-evaluative
information, whereas openness to experience has very
little to do with self-evaluative information and is not
necessarily an interpersonal construct (it entails a
general mental openness to information and experience
that is not necessarily tied to interactions with others),
and thus we expect expressed humility to be more tied to
relational outcomes in organizations compared with
openness to experience.
Honesty-humility Part of the HEXACO personality model
(Ashton and Lee 2008), encompassing the
subcomponents of sincerity, fairness, greed
avoidance, and modesty.
Honesty-humility includes some important prosocial
characteristics that are likely to be related to the
expressed humility construct but does not capture the
core elements of willingness to view oneself accurately,
teachability, and appreciation of others.
Learning goal orientation An adaptive approach to task situations
associated with the motivation to
understand and master the task rather than
to display or prove competence
(Dweck 2000).
Learning goal orientation describes cognitive and
behavioral response patterns in achievement situations,
whereas expressed humility is manifest in a broad range
of social situations that may or may not have direct
achievement implications. LGO reflects a desire (i.e., an
internal motivation or cognition) to develop new
competencies, master new situations, and acquire new
skills; expressed humility reflects behaviors that reflect a
pursuit of accurate self-awareness and appreciation of
others’ strengths, in addition to learning and
development.
Core self-evaluation A higher-order trait that represents a baseline
or comprehensive view of the self and
comprises the four lower-order constructs of
self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy,
internal locus of control, and emotional
stability (Judge et al. 2003).
Generalized self-efficacy reflects optimistic self-beliefs,
whereas expressed humility reflects a motivation for an
accurate self-view. Expressed humility and internal locus
of control also share theoretical roots with regard to
determinism, as humility allows individuals to believe they
can improve their personal weaknesses. However,
internal locus of control focuses more on beliefs about
what happens to us externally, whereas expressed
humility is more focused on issues dealing with personal
development. Practically speaking, core self-evaluation is
important for motivation and persistence (locus of
control, self-efficacy) and consistency in performance
(emotional stability), but it says little with regard to how
viewing others may influence workplace performance via
social learning, nor whether such self-views are an
accurate portrayal of self-abilities (i.e., hyper-CSE has
been suggested as a proxy for hubris; see Hiller and
Hambrick 2005).
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1522 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
Second, in addition to using existing scales as prox-
ies for humility, others have tried to develop self-report
measures of humility. Attempting to circumvent the
social desirability bias of measuring humility via self-
reporting, Emmons (as reported in Tangney 2002) cre-
ated a scale using a forced-choice format. However,
results from the initial scale development analysis
showed prohibitively low internal consistency. Part of
the reason for this is that serious problems exist concep-
tually with assessing humility via self-report. For peo-
ple to consider themselves exceptionally humble seems
paradoxical to the construct. Because of these problems,
several scholars have suggested that assessing humil-
ity through close observers may be the ideal approach
(Exline et al. 2004, Davis et al. 2010). For instance,
Richards (1992) argued that although those who actually
possess humility are not likely to attribute this virtue
to themselves, close others may be able to observe and
report on this virtue. Observer reports of personality
have been shown to have a high degree of consensus
(John and Robins 1993, Kenny 1994) and a high level of
accuracy (agreement with self-reports), even when expo-
sure to the person rated is brief or indirect (Gosling et al.
2002). We report on our efforts to develop an observer-
report measure of expressed humility below.
Study 1: Developing a Measure of
Expressed Humility
Method
Participants and Procedures. Five samples were used
to develop and validate items for a new expressed humil-
ity scale. Samples A, B, and C consisted of undergrad-
uate students from a large northwestern U.S. university.
Sample D consisted of employees of a large U.S. health
services organization, and Sample E consisted of full-
time employees from a commercial subject pool. Details
regarding each of these samples and the purpose(s) for
using each sample are provided in Table 2.
Table 2 Characteristics of the Study 1 Samples
Sample Source N Gender Average age Ethnicity Work experience Analysis
A Upper-level 164 52% male 21 50% Caucasian N/A Exploratory factor
undergraduate analysis, discriminant
business students validity
B Upper-level 236 49% male 22 59% Caucasian 2.8 years Confirmatory factor
undergraduate analysis
business students
C Upper-level 124 52% male 22 50% Caucasian 3.3 years Test-retest reliability
undergraduate
business students
D Employees of a large 511 63% male 48 79% Caucasian 4.7 years Confirmatory factor
health maintenance (organization tenure) analysis
organization
E Full-time employees 263 37% male 39 79% Caucasian 13.2 years Discriminant validity
in a commercial
subject pool
Instrument Development. Based on our definition
of expressed humility, we generated items that were
screened to ensure they were content valid, clear, and
concise. This process resulted in an initial list of
32 items, and our sample sizes allowed for at least a
5:1 subject to item ratio. Items were scaled using a Lik-
ert format ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly
agree. In Samples A and B, participants were asked to
type the initials of an individual they knew very well
on the survey and then to assess this person on the
expressed humility items. Sample C participants were
asked to rate members of their student project teams,
and Samples D and E participants were asked to rate
their immediate supervisors.
To further establish construct validity, we admin-
istered to Samples A and E scales measuring con-
structs that are expected to be related (for nomolog-
ical validity) and unrelated (for discriminant validity)
to the expressed humility construct. Specifically, the
Sample A survey included openness, conscientiousness,
emotional stability, extroversion (Barrick and Mount
1991), modesty (Peterson and Seligman 2004), nar-
cissism (Margolis and Thomas 1980), and core self-
evaluations (Judge et al. 2003). The Sample E survey
included the measures of honesty-humility (Ashton and
Lee 2008) and learning goal orientation (VanDeWalle
1997). Similar to the expressed humility scale, items rep-
resenting the Big Five, modesty, narcissism, and core
self-evaluations were adjusted to match an observer-
report format (i.e., “this person” was used in each
descriptive statement), because previous research has
shown the other-report approach to rating personality is
not only valid but often more accurate than self-report
measures (see Kolar et al. 1996).
Results
Instrument Analysis. We conducted exploratory factor
analyses using data from Sample A for the expressed
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1523
Table 3 Expressed Humility Scale Descriptive Statistics, Loadings, and Reliability
Scale item Item mean Item SD Factor loadings Alpha if item deleted
1. This person actively seeks feedback, 2080 1015 0074 0094
even if it is critical.
2. This person admits it when they don’t know 3020 1012 0085 0093
how to do something.
3. This person acknowledges when others have more 3028 1007 0080 0094
knowledge and skills than him- or herself.
4. This person takes notice of others’ strengths. 3037 1000 0080 0094
5. This person often compliments others on their strengths. 3005 1001 0074 0094
6. This person shows appreciation for the unique 3019 1000 0079 0094
contributions of others.
7. This person is willing to learn from others. 3039 1006 0083 0093
8. This person is open to the ideas of others. 3029 1003 0083 0093
9. This person is open to the advice of others. 3035 1008 0088 0093
humility scales. Through an iterative process, we
sequentially removed items with factor loadings below
0.20 or cross loadings above 0.40. This process resulted
in a nine-item, three-factor solution. Scale items, fac-
tor loadings, item means, and standard deviations are
presented in Table 3. The reliability for the resulting
expressed humility scales was = 0094. Items repre-
senting the expressed humility components of willing-
ness to view oneself accurately (items 1, 2, and 3),
appreciation of others’ strengths (items 4, 5, and 6),
and teachability (items 7, 8, and 9) were retained in
the refined scale, suggesting a three-factor structure. As
a face validity test, we followed the guidelines in pre-
vious studies to conduct a sorting task (Anderson and
Gerbing 1991). We recruited 10 content experts (i.e.,
professors and researchers who have been involved with
research on humility) to sort these nine items along the
three subdimensions of expressed humility, and, as a
foil, we included nine items from the modesty scale
(Peterson and Seligman 2004) and nine items from a
Machiavellian scale (Jackson Personality Inventory; see
Jackson 1977). After we provided a definition for each
construct, participants were asked to assign each item to
one of the construct categories according to their respec-
tive construct definitions. In all these codings, there was
only one instance where an expressed humility item
was coded as belonging to the modesty construct and
only two instances where a modesty item was coded
as belonging to the expressed humility construct. There
were no other cross-categorizations across constructs.
We viewed this as strong evidence for the face valid-
ity of the expressed humility items and the underlying
dimensions.
Using data from Sample B, we conducted a confir-
matory factor analysis to test the factor structure of the
nine-item expressed humility scale. Two competing a
priori models were tested: a one-factor solution and a
three-factor solution with the three subcomponents load-
ing onto a second-order general expressed humility fac-
tor. Following the guidelines outlined by Kline (1998,
p. 130) we report four tests of fit: chi-squared, good-
ness of fit (GFI), nonnormed fit index (NNFI), and
standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR). The
one-factor model had a chi-square value of 1406.05
(p < 000013 df = 27), a GFI of 0.77, an NNFI of 0.92,
and an SRMR of 0.05, suggesting moderate fit. In con-
trast, the three-factor model loading onto a second-order
factor had much better fit, with a chi-square value of
410.47 (p < 000013 df = 24), a GFI of 0.93, an NNFI
of 0.97, and an SRMR of 0.03. These data support our
conceptualization that expressed humility is reflected by
three underlying, interconnected components. In other
words, our expressed humility measure is a reflective
measure that captures three manifest indicators of the
latent expressed humility construct (see Edwards and
Bagozzi 2000).
The nine-item expressed humility scale was admin-
istered to a third sample, Sample C, to analyze the
reliability of the scales over time. The expressed humil-
ity scores from Time 1 and Time 2 (approximately one
month later) were related (r = 00561 p < 00001), indi-
cating evidence of test-retest reliability. The expressed
humility scale was then administered to Sample D to
replicate this factor structure with a field sample. Simi-
lar to the procedure conducted with Sample B, we tested
a one-factor solution and three-factor solution loading
onto a higher-order factor. The one-factor model again
achieved moderate fit, with a chi-square value of 619.47
(p < 00001; df = 27), a GFI of 0.77, an NNFI of 0.82,
and an SRMR of 0.06. In contrast, the three-factor
model loading onto a second-order factor had much bet-
ter fit, yielding a chi-square value of 213.98 (p < 00001;
df = 24), a GFI of 0.90, an NNFI of 0.93, and an SRMR
of 0.04. Internal consistency reliability for this work
sample was equivalent to the reliabilities of the student
sample ( = 0095 and = 0094, respectively). Taken as a
whole, these results suggest that the nine-item expressed
humility scale had sufficient reliability to proceed with
other analyses.
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1524 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
Table 4 Study 1 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Variable Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Expressed humility 3021 0085 400945
2. Modesty 2066 0066 0062
∗∗
400845
3. Narcissism 1038 0030 0063
∗∗
0069
∗∗
400875
4. Conscientiousness 3021 0079 0028
∗∗
0022
0035
∗∗
400905
5. Extraversion 3004 0087 0011 0006 0010 0014 400885
6. Openness to experience 3026 0057 0031
∗∗
0009 0035 0034
∗∗
0006 400825
7. Emotional stability 3054 0073 0049
∗∗
0032
∗∗
0050
∗∗
0017 0008 0019 400935
8. Core self-evaluation 3032 0059 0034
∗∗
0010 0041
∗∗
0042
∗∗
0012 0051
∗∗
0055
∗∗
400865
9. Honesty-humility
a
3021 0077 0055
∗∗
0068
∗∗
0028
∗∗
400905
10. Learning goal orientation
a
3054 0077 0063
∗∗
0011 0072
∗∗
400905
Notes. n = 164. Scale alpha reliabilities are given on the diagonal (in parentheses).
a
Honesty-humility and learning goal orientation scores assessed using Sample E 4n = 2635, which also assessed expressed humility,
modesty, and openness to experience.
p < 0005;
∗∗
p < 0001.
Nomological Network Analysis. As a test of dis-
criminant validity, we conducted two additional factor
analyses to explore whether items from the expressed
humility measure loaded with items of conceptually
related constructs (see DeVellis 2011). First, using data
from Sample A, the resulting nine items from the
expressed humility scale were entered into a factor anal-
ysis along with the items representing trait extraversion,
conscientiousness, emotional stability, openness to expe-
rience, modesty, core self-evaluations, and narcissism.
Using data from Sample F, we entered the nine-item
expressed humility scale with the honesty-humility and
learning goal orientation items. Across these analyses,
only two items from existing scales had cross loadings
above 0.40 that loaded onto the expressed humility con-
struct. We take this as strong evidence of discriminant
validity for the expressed humility construct.
Table 4 reports the bivariate relationships between
expressed humility and the traits of modesty, narcissism,
components of the Big Five, and core self-evaluation.
These relationships were in line with our expecta-
tions. Specifically, narcissism was negatively related to
expressed humility (r = 0063, p < 0001). Modesty was
positively related (r = 00621 p < 0001). Expressed humil-
ity was also positively related to openness to experi-
ence (r = 0031, p < 0001), emotional stability (r = 0049,
p < 0001), conscientiousness (r = 0028, p < 0001), and
core self-evaluations (r = 0034, p < 0001). Using data
from Sample E, expressed humility was shown to be
positively related to honesty-humility (r = 0055, p <
0001) and learning goal orientation (r = 0063, p < 0001)
(for convenience, correlations from both Samples A and
E are reported in the same table). From a nomological
validity standpoint, the other-report expressed humility
scale seems to reflect a construct that fits within the the-
oretical network of constructs we have discussed.
Discussion
This study provides initial support for our conceptual-
ization of expressed humility, its validity as observed
by others, its relationship with other constructs, its
underlying dimensionality, and its positive nature. This
study also revealed initial support for external validity,
as the factor structure and alpha reliabilities were equiv-
alent from student and employee samples. However, this
study lends no insight into the predictive validity of
expressed humility. Thus, building off this initial evi-
dence of construct validity, we seek in Study 2 to further
establish expressed humility as an important construct
in organizational contexts by examining its predictive
validity.
Study 2: Relationship of Expressed
Humility with Individual Performance and
Quality of Team Member Contribution
In Study 2, we sought to further establish the valid-
ity of the expressed humility construct by examining
(a) whether expressed humility enhances the quality
of one’s contribution on a team and (b) the incre-
mental predictive validity of expressed humility over
other major predictors of individual performance. In
line with our discussion of the importance of expressed
humility within organizational contexts, we expected
that expressed humility would foster more meaningful
and satisfying interrelations with others (Means et al.
1990, Exline et al. 2004). Increasingly, firms are orga-
nizing workers into teams in hopes of fostering syn-
ergies of experience, skills, and knowledge, as well
as to encourage mutual mentoring and peer regulation.
Research has shown, however, that the intended bene-
fits of organizing around teams are often not realized
because team member characteristics may starkly con-
trast with expressed humility. Members who display
characteristics such as self-enhancement and arrogance
are punished by other team members because of their
disruptiveness to group functioning (see Anderson et al.
2006, Horowitz et al. 2006). In contrast, we expect that
the behaviors of expressed humility (i.e., owning up to
personal mistakes and weaknesses, being receptive to
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1525
feedback, and acknowledging the strengths and contri-
butions of other team members) facilitate effective team
functioning and make it more likely that team members
will view humble team members as quality contributors
to the team’s efforts.
Hypothesis 1. Individual expressed humility is posi-
tively related to team member contribution ratings.
We also expect that expressed humility captures some
unique adaptive behaviors important for individual per-
formance that are not captured by common perfor-
mance predictors. Specifically, we expect that the three
expressed humility dimensions, as measured, represent
behaviors that facilitate individual performance beyond
conscientiousness, generalized self-efficacy, and general
mental ability. First, humble people manifest a will-
ingness to see themselves accurately, to work toward
an unexaggerated understanding of personal strengths
and weaknesses. We expect that this enhanced self-
understanding about strengths and limitations should
impact decisions about how much time and effort one
should allocate to performance-related tasks. For exam-
ple, those who overestimate their ability may allocate
less time and effort than is needed, resulting in miss-
ing deadlines or sacrificing quality to finish tasks on
time, thus leading to decreased performance. However,
those who are willing to see themselves accurately will
have a more precise sense of their abilities, leading to a
more realistic view of how much time and effort will be
needed to fulfill performance expectations. Humble peo-
ple with this self-understanding will also “play to their
strengths” where possible and seek help or feedback on
the aspects of the task where they might be weak.
Second, the teachability component of expressed
humility suggests that humility is associated with open-
ness to feedback. Humble individuals are more likely to
learn from their mistakes and take remedial action after
low performance, leading to higher overall performance
over time. Third, humility involves an appreciation of
the strengths of others. In a performance context, peo-
ple possessing high levels of humility should be less
likely to discount or devalue the strengths or high per-
formance of those around them. Therefore, it may be
that those who are humble are more likely to notice
and benefit from the positive modeling of high perform-
ers. Combined, we expect expressed humility to be pos-
itively associated with overall performance. Moreover,
the positive performance benefits of expressed humility
just discussed are not captured by general mental ability,
conscientiousness, and self-efficacy, three of the most
robust individual difference predictors of performance.
Thus, we hypothesize the following.
Hypothesis 2. Individual expressed humility posi-
tively predicts individual performance, over and above
generalized self-efficacy, conscientiousness, and general
mental ability.
Method
Participants and Procedures. Subjects were 144 stu-
dents from three sections of an upper-level undergrad-
uate management course. In all, 51% of the subjects
were male, 51% were Caucasian, the mean age was 22.8,
and the mean level of work experience was 42 months.
The class entailed completing a number of assessments
throughout the quarter for which the students would
be awarded course credit. Individual performance was
based on the average of individual grades on tests and
assignments (independent of team-related grades). This
performance metric represents an objective proxy of par-
ticipants’ effort on assignments and tests throughout
the course period and, we propose, reflects participants’
diligence, timeliness, preparedness, and thoroughness.
In the first month of the quarter, students were asked
to complete an online self-assessment of general self-
efficacy (Schwarzer and Jerusalem 1995) and conscien-
tiousness (Barrick and Mount 1991). Sample items for
general self-efficacy include “I can always manage to
solve difficult problems if I try hard enough” and “It is
easy for me to stick to my aims and accomplish my
goals.” Sample items for conscientiousness include “I try
to perform all the tasks assigned to me conscientiously”
and “When I make a commitment, I can always be
counted on to follow through. Both assessments were
measured on a five-point scale. One month later, stu-
dents were asked to complete the Wonderlic Personnel
Test (Wonderlic 1973, Dodrill 1983) as a measure of
general mental ability.
On the first day of class, students were divided into
teams for the purpose of working together on team-
related assignments throughout the quarter and to com-
plete a culminating final team project that entailed a
written report and presentation at the end of the quar-
ter. The structuring of this team project experience, and
the frequent interaction it fostered, was created to pro-
vide the psychological (both mundane and experimen-
tal) realism to approximate a real project team in the
work context (see Colquitt 2008). Thus, far from being
a short-lived hypothetical lab experience, the students
were in real, 10-week-long project teams.
Other-report expressed humility was gathered at the
middle of the term and team member contribution rat-
ings at the end of the term (after completing the team
project but before they knew their team project or indi-
vidual grades). Each group member rated each teammate
on the expressed humility items validated in Study 1.
Students were assured that their responses would be con-
fidential (i.e., their team members would not know how
they rated them) and that their ratings would not impact
their team grades.
Demographic data, such as gender, race, age, and
amount of work experience, were gathered as controls.
Because we were trying to establish the predictive-
ness of expressed humility beyond established constructs
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1526 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
(especially those we have identified as conceptually
related), we also included the measures of core self-
evaluation (Judge et al. 2003) and openness to expe-
rience (McCrae and Costa 2004) with the intention of
controlling for these constructs. We controlled for gen-
der because past research suggests that females show
more humility than males (Furnham et al. 2002). We
controlled for age and work experience because scholars
have suggested that humility might develop with accrued
life experience (Tangney 2000), and we controlled for
race because of the potential differences in the valuing
and expressing of humility across collectivistic and indi-
vidualistic cultures (Morris et al. 2005). Using a five-
item scale, team members rated each other on the quality
of their contributions to the team project. The items were
anchored to a five-point Likert scale (1 = very low; 5 =
very high); sample items from this scale included “The
quality of this team member’s contribution to the team
project was 0 0 0 0 and “The overall value that this team
member added to the team was 0 0 0 0 ( = 0090). These
contribution scores were averaged and used as the crite-
rion variable for testing Hypothesis 1. Because each par-
ticipant’s midterm humility score and end-of-term team
member contribution score were based on the average of
his or her team members’ ratings, as a robustness check,
we ran analyses only including groups with an r
wg
of
more than 0.80. However, the results were equivalent
regardless of whether we included groups with these low
r
wg
scores, so we report our results with the full sample.
Results
Table 5 contains descriptive statistics and bivariate cor-
relations for all the study variables. Expressed humil-
ity showed a significant positive relationship with team
contribution (r = 00331 p < 0001) and individual per-
formance (r = 00351 p < 0001). Table 6 reports the
results of regression analyses to test the hypothesis
Table 5 Study 2 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Variable Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Gender
a
0048 0050
2. Race
b
0052 0050 0020
∗∗
3. Age 21067 2090 0003 0001
4. Work experience 38010 31070 0002 0009 0054
5. Openness 3032 0050 0004 0008 0005 0001
6. Core self-evaluation 3079 0058 0002 0019
0003 0003 0006 400755
7. Self-efficacy 3092 0050 0001 0011 0003 0002 0028
∗∗
0070
∗∗
400775
8. Conscientiousness 3083 0051 0015
0010 0001 0007 0003 0036
∗∗
0036
∗∗
400845
9. General mental ability 26065 5079 0026
∗∗
0018
0020
∗∗
0003 0010 0017
0031
∗∗
0007
10. Expressed humility 3084 0039 0017
0009 0017
0009 0003 0012 0008 0018
0002 400945
11. Team contribution 4018 0074 0023
∗∗
0003 0010 0015 0007 0008 0002 0030 0011 0033
∗∗
400905
12. Individual performance 0085 0010 0006 0005 0028
∗∗
0018
∗∗
0002 0015
0007 0014
0016
0035
∗∗
0070
∗∗∗
Notes. n = 144. Scale alpha reliabilities are given on the diagonal (in parentheses).
a
Represents a dummy variable: male = 0, female = 1.
b
Represents a dummy variable: non-Caucasian = 0, Caucasian = 1.
p < 0005;
∗∗
p < 0001.
that expressed humility would predict team mem-
ber contribution ratings and individual performance
above and beyond conscientiousness, generalized self-
efficacy, and general mental ability. Comparing Model 2
to Model 3 under the “Team contribution” column,
after controlling for age, gender, race, work expe-
rience, core self-evaluation, and openness to experi-
ence, expressed humility positively predicted quality
of team contribution above and beyond conscientious-
ness, self-efficacy, and general mental ability ( = 0034,
ãR
2
= 0010, p < 00001). Thus, Hypothesis 1 was sup-
ported. Comparing Model 2 to Model 3 under the “Indi-
vidual performance” column, after controlling for age,
gender, race, and work experience, expressed humil-
ity positively predicted individual performance above
and beyond conscientiousness, self-efficacy, and general
mental ability ( = 0023, ãR
2
= 0005, p < 0001). Thus,
Hypothesis 2 was supported.
Post Hoc Analyses. Recent research has shown that
some individual characteristics can compensate for low
general mental ability on performance tasks. In one
example, Côté and Miners (2006) found a compen-
satory effect for emotional intelligence such that the
relationship between emotional intelligence and perfor-
mance became stronger as general mental ability became
weaker. Part of the rationale for this compensatory
model was that emotional intelligence facilitates aspects
of performance that raw general mental ability does
not, such as identifying and regulating self and oth-
ers’ emotions in working toward cooperative goals. We
have discussed how humility may facilitate a more real-
istic allocation of personal resources toward complet-
ing tasks, the likelihood of taking remedial action after
receiving negative feedback, and enhanced social learn-
ing. Thus, humility may also compensate for low general
mental ability. To examine this possibility, we created
an interaction term using the expressed humility and
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1527
Table 6 Results of Regression Analyses for Comparing the Predictiveness of Expressed Humility, Conscientiousness,
Generalized Self-Efficacy, and General Mental Ability on Individual Performance and Performance Improvement
Team contribution Individual performance
Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Model 7 Model 8
Controls
Gender
a
0022
0023
0022
0022
0021
0018
0017
0017
Race 0006 0001 0001 0002 0008 0003 0003 0002
Age 0013 0007 0012 0012 0003 0003 0001 0001
Work experience 0023
0021
0018 0019
0000 0005 0007 0006
Openness 0002 0005 0006 0002 0005 0003 0003 0001
Core self-evaluation 0016
0006 0010 0011 0017
0007 0010 0010
Predictors
Self-efficacy 0012 0010 0013 0013 0012 0016
Conscientiousness 0033
0026
∗∗
0027
∗∗
0039
∗∗∗
0037
∗∗
0038
∗∗∗
General mental ability 0021
0020
0021
0017
0016
0017
Expressed humility 0034
∗∗∗
0036
∗∗∗
0023
∗∗
0027
∗∗
Interaction terms
Expressed humility × General mental ability 0021
0023
∗∗
F 2022
3024
∗∗
4089
∗∗∗
5023
∗∗∗
1056 3020
∗∗
3074
∗∗∗
4030
∗∗∗
R
2
0011 0022 0032 0036 0008 0021 0026 0031
ãR
2
0011
0010
∗∗∗
0004
0013
∗∗
0005
∗∗
0005
∗∗
Note. n = 144.
a
All values reflect standardized beta coefficients.
p < 0010;
p < 0005;
∗∗
p < 0001;
∗∗∗
p < 00001.
general mental ability variables. As shown in Model 8
in Table 6, the interaction term accounted for significant
additional variance, suggesting that expressed humility
interacts with general mental ability to predict individ-
ual performance ( = 00231 ãR
2
= 00051 p < 0001).
In Figure 1, the slopes suggest that expressed humil-
ity has a compensatory effect on performance for those
with lower general mental ability. In other words, though
expressed humility had a relatively small positive impact
on performance for those with high general mental abil-
ity, expressed humility made a considerable difference in
performance for those with low general mental ability.
Figure 1 The Compensatory Effect of Expressed Humility on
the Relationship Between Intelligence and
Individual Performance
74
76
80
78
84
82
86
88
Low humility High humility
Individual performance
a
Low general mental ability
High general mental ability
a
Represents percentage points.
To separately test one of the reasons why expressed
humility may compensate for poor performance—that
those with humility will be more likely to take reme-
dial action to improve poor performance (teachability)—
we conducted an additional analysis to assess whether
humility predicted improvement in test performance over
time. All student participants took a midterm (test 1) and
a final exam (test 2) during the quarter. The overall class
averages for the midterm and final exams were equiva-
lent (M = 0081 and M = 00801 respectively). We exam-
ined whether expressed humility predicted performance
on test 2 while controlling for performance on test 1.
In our hierarchical regression analysis, age, gender, race,
work experience, and test 1 scores were controlled for
in block 1. General self-efficacy, conscientiousness, and
general mental ability (the common performance predic-
tors) were entered in block 2; expressed humility was
entered in block 3. Compared with self-efficacy, consci-
entiousness, and general mental ability, expressed humil-
ity was the strongest predictor of individual performance
improvement, explaining 9% of the variance in improve-
ment in test scores above and beyond the other variables
( = 00391 ãR
2
= 00091 p < 0001).
Discussion
The main purpose of Study 2 was to further validate
the expressed humility scale by analyzing its criterion
validity. We hypothesized that expressed humility would
enhance one’s ability to work well with others and would
enhance individual performance. As expected, expressed
humility was predictive of team contribution ratings and
individual performance beyond the related constructs of
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1528 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
core self-evaluation and openness to experience and the
common performance predictors of self-efficacy, consci-
entiousness, and general mental ability. We also uncov-
ered an interesting interactive effective with intelligence.
In addition to being predictive of individual perfor-
mance above and beyond common performance pre-
dictors, expressed humility was shown to have a com-
pensating effect on performance for those with lower
general mental ability. Unlike team contribution rat-
ings, individual performance was derived from a source
that was independent of expressed humility ratings and
thus is a key strength of this study. Expressed humility
was hypothesized to predict unique variance in perfor-
mance because it captures adaptive behaviors that are
not reflected in trait conscientiousness, self-efficacy, or
general mental ability. Specifically, expressed humility
reflects receptiveness to feedback (teachability), better-
informed decisions about attributes needed to meet task
performance expectations (viewing oneself accurately),
and receptivity to the positive modeling of peers (appre-
ciation of others’ strengths). This study also showed that
expressed humility predicts unique variance in perfor-
mance beyond the constructs of core self-evaluation and
openness to experience; this suggests that the concep-
tual differences between expressed humility and these
related constructs have practical implications for predict-
ing important outcomes. In addition, relative to all other
study constructs, expressed humility was the best pre-
dictor of performance improvement.
However, Study 2 also had some limitations. First,
because both humility and team member contributions
were assessed the same way, this relationship could be
a methodological artifact, though common method vari-
ance would not apply to the performance dependent
variable. Also, it should be noted that we have no evi-
dence regarding some of the mediating mechanisms we
mention in our hypothesis rationale, such as feedback-
seeking behaviors and mimetic behaviors.
Organizational Validity
Study 3: The Influence of Leader-Expressed
Humility on Follower Outcomes
Study 1 provided support for the three-dimensional
nature of the expressed humility construct (willing-
ness to see the self accurately, appreciation of others’
strengths, and teachability) and established evidence for
construct and nomological validity. Study 2 provided
initial evidence for the predictive validity of expressed
humility by testing its connection to team contribution
ratings and individual performance. Building off Stud-
ies 1 and 2 and in an effort to further establish the
predictive and external validity of the new expressed
humility construct, Study 3 aimed to provide evidence
that expressed humility positively influences important
outcomes within real organizational contexts. Given the
proposed interpersonal nature of expressed humility, and
because much of the current literature has speculated
about the importance of leader humility in general, in
this study we focused specifically on the implications of
expressed humility in leader–follower relations.
Recent qualitative research suggests that expressed
humility might be an important leader attribute. In a
study that explored the behaviors, effects, and contin-
gencies of leader humility (Owens and Hekman 2012),
humility was found to have important implications for
learning, employee engagement, and employee job sat-
isfaction. As qualitative insights, these outcomes have
not yet been confirmed by quantitative empirical study.
Thus, in this study we focused on a quantitative anal-
ysis of the influence of leader-expressed humility on
team learning orientation and employee job engagement
(Path 1) and on employee job satisfaction and voluntary
turnover (Path 2).
Path 1: Team Learning Orientation and Team Mem-
ber Engagement. Team learning orientation refers to
“a shared perception of team goals related to learning
and competence development” (Bunderson and Sutcliffe
2003, p. 553). In other words, it refers to the degree
to which a team is oriented toward proactive learning,
reflecting a desire to develop new competencies, mas-
ter new situations, and acquire new skills (see Table 1).
Although learning orientation can be considered a sta-
ble characteristic, research has also shown that it can
be primed (i.e., fostered, reinforced) by situational fac-
tors. Two important situational factors that have been
shown to influence goal orientation are leadership (i.e.,
transformational; see Gong et al. 2009) and authority
relations (Ames 1992, Turner et al. 2002). Specifically,
different leadership approaches have been shown to
sanction or discourage learning orientations and behav-
iors in teams. Although “authoritarian” leaders have
been found to inhibit team member learning behavior
(Edmondson 1999), we propose that leaders who express
humility will (1) look for, (2) recognize, and (3) appre-
ciate the development of new competencies and skills.
They will enact the humble behaviors of admitting their
own mistakes and limitations, modeling teachability and
openness to learning, and acknowledging the strengths
and contributions of team members, which will foster a
team climate in which team members are more focused
on development and more willing to risk engaging in
learning behaviors.
More specifically, leaders who show the behaviors of
humility help legitimize learning and personal develop-
ment (i.e., send a signal to employees that it is okay to
be “in process” here; see Owens and Hekman 2012) and
foster openness, trust, and recognition, which have been
shown to be antecedents of learning goal orientation (see
Bunderson and Sutcliffe 2003 for a review). Therefore,
we propose the following hypothesis.
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1529
Hypothesis 3. Leader-expressed humility is posi-
tively related to team learning orientation.
In addition to team orientations, our review of the lit-
erature also suggested that leaders’ display of humility
has positive motivational effects on individual employ-
ees. One specific positive effect of leader humility that
was evident from our review is enhanced employee
job engagement. Job engagement reflects the degree to
which individuals invest their entire selves in their work
roles (i.e., physical, emotional, and cognitive; see Rich
et al. 2010) and has been captured by the level of absorp-
tion, vigor, and dedication an individual has toward his
work (Schaufeli and Bakker 2004). We know from past
research that when leaders show what may be viewed
as a lack of humility (taking all the credit for success,
overestimating their contributions relative to others, not
listening), followers disengage and lose their motiva-
tion to work hard under their leader (see Burke 2006,
Lubit 2002). However, we expect that leaders who show
humility by acknowledging the strengths and contribu-
tions of followers and being teachable will help fos-
ter the preconditions for employee engagement, such as
“dignity, self-appreciation, and a sense of value” (Kahn
1990, p. 705). This connection between leader humility
and employee job engagement was also found in recent
qualitative research (Owens and Hekman 2012).
2
In light
of this review, we propose the following hypothesis.
Hypothesis 4. Leader-expressed humility is posi-
tively related to employee engagement.
Furthermore, we propose that the group climate of
learning and development—which we suggest is fos-
tered by humble leader behaviors—will enable indi-
vidual group members to invest more of themselves
in their job roles (i.e., be more engaged) because an
intrinsic orientation toward learning has been associ-
ated with increased engagement in past research (Meece
et al. 1988). Moreover, Kahn (1990) suggested that both
supportive leadership and the tone of the work group
are important antecedents of job engagement. Thus, we
expect that the effect of leader-expressed humility on
employee job engagement is through the team learning
orientation that humble leader behaviors help foster.
Hypothesis 5. The positive relationship between
leader-expressed humility and employee job engagement
is mediated by team learning orientation.
Path 2: Job Satisfaction and Voluntary Turnover. We
also theorize that leader humility will have a positive
effect on employee job satisfaction. There is substan-
tial literature showing that the leader has a major influ-
ence on his or her employees’ overall job satisfac-
tion (see meta-analysis from Dirks and Ferrin 2002).
Insights from qualitative research and theoretical work
on leader humility also point specifically to a con-
nection between leader-expressed humility and positive
employee job attitudes, such as job satisfaction (Owens
and Hekman 2012, Morris et al. 2005). Among other
things, job satisfaction is shaped in large part by favor-
able perceptions of supervisors or leaders (Russell et al.
2004). Second, almost all measures of job satisfaction
(e.g., the Job Descriptive Index) have a dimension that
reflects one’s satisfaction with supervision. Nearly three-
quarters of employees report that the worst aspect of
their job is their immediate boss (Hogan and Kaiser
2005), and many of the complaints employees have
about their leaders appear to reflect the opposite of the
proposed dimensions of expressed humility (i.e., arro-
gance (Dotlich and Cairo 2003); devaluing the opinions
or views of others (Fulmer and Conger 2004); thinking
they have all the answers or having an inflated self-view
(Finkelstein 2003)). In contrast, we expect that leaders
who present a realistic view of themselves (i.e., aware-
ness of weaknesses and mistakes), who are open to the
ideas of others, and who give employees due credit for
their contributions and strengths will help employees
generally feel better about their overall job experience.
Hypothesis 6. Leader-expressed humility is posi-
tively related to employee job satisfaction.
We also know from numerous studies that the rela-
tionship with the boss is a critical factor in determining
whether employees choose to stay or not. One’s “imme-
diate supervisor is perhaps one of the most influential
people in his or her work life, influencing job perfor-
mance, job attitudes, well-being, and attachment (Perry
et al. 2010, p. 1145). Indeed, leadership quality has been
shown to be a powerful predictor of employee turnover
(see Richardson and Vandenberg 2005) and in some
cases just as powerful a predictor as employee attitudes
(see Ferris 1985). In addition, past studies have shown
that leaders who recognize the potential of employees
and have a relationship of mutual disclosure (factors
that reflect dimensions of leader-expressed humility) are
more likely to retain their followers (Graen et al. 1982).
Thus, we expected the following.
Hypothesis 7. Leader-expressed humility is nega-
tively related to voluntary employee turnover.
The question, of course, is how leader-expressed
humility more specifically influences the desire to stay
or leave. We would suggest that job satisfaction medi-
ates leader-expressed humility and employee turnover
for the following reasons. First, job satisfaction is the
strongest single predictor of turnover. Starting with
March and Simon (1958), and including the meta-
analysis by Griffeth et al. (2000) and the more recent
review in the Academy of Management Annals by
Holtom et al. (2008), job satisfaction has emerged as
the primary conduit through which other variables influ-
ence turnover (Hom and Griffeth 1995). Job satisfaction
has been shown to mediate relationships between many
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1530 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
aspects of the job (e.g., compensation, nature of the
work, advancement opportunities, work group relations)
and turnover (Boswell et al. 2005). Dickter et al. (1996,
p. 706) go so far as to say “in most studies of turnover
in the organizational literature job satisfaction is the
key psychological construct leading to turnover. Given
our hypothesis suggesting that leader humility fosters
increased job satisfaction, and because job satisfaction
is one of the strongest predictors of voluntary turnover,
we propose that the relationship between leader humility
and voluntary turnover is mediated by job satisfaction.
Hypothesis 8. The negative relationship between
leader humility and voluntary turnover is mediated by
employee job satisfaction.
Method
Sample and Procedure. We sampled employees of
a large U.S midwestern health services organization.
This sample contained 704 employees rating 218 lead-
ers (mean group size was 3.56 employees); 72% of the
employees were female and 70% were Caucasian. The
average age and tenure under their current leader of this
sample was 37 years and 12.5 months, respectively.
We were given permission to include our measures
of the study constructs with the organization’s own
annual confidential assessment. Each employee in the
organization was invited through email to complete a
voluntary, two-part annual organizational assessment.
Parts 1 and 2 were administered online approximately
one month apart, and the response rates were 67% and
54%, respectively. In part 1, participants were asked to
rate their immediate leader or supervisor on the nine
humility items developed in Study 1. Part 1 also con-
tained questions asking for demographic information
(gender, age, race, and tenure under current leader).
Part 2 contained a 5-item team learning orientation scale
(Bunderson and Sutcliffe 2003), a 12-item job engage-
ment scale (Schaufeli et al. 2002), and a 7-item job sat-
isfaction scale (Cook et al. 1981). Sample items from
the team learning orientation scale include “My team
sees learning and developing skills as very important”
and “My team likes challenging and difficult assign-
ments that teach new things. Sample items from the job
engagement scale include “I am immersed in my work”
and At my job, I am very resilient, mentally. Sam-
ple items from the job satisfaction scale include All in
all, how satisfied are you with your job?” and All in
all, how satisfied are you with the persons in your work
group?” Humility and job satisfaction were measured
on five-point scales (5 = strongly agree and 5 = very
satisfied, respectively). Team learning orientation and
job engagement were measured on a seven-point agree-
ment scale (7 = strongly agree). As noted above, team
learning orientation, job satisfaction, and job engage-
ment were measured one month after leader-expressed
humility in an effort to reduce common method variance
through temporal separation (Podsakoff et al. 2003).
Approximately four months after the part 2 assessment
was administered, we requested and received voluntary
turnover data from the company for all participants.
Analyses. Because of the multilevel and nested nature
of the data (i.e., groups of employees rating their leader),
we tested Hypotheses 4–8 using hierarchical linear mod-
eling (i.e., HLM 7.0). We used regression analyses to
test Hypothesis 3, given that leader humility and team
learning goal orientation were both group-level vari-
ables. The average r
wg
values for the level 2 variables,
leader humility and team learning goal-orientation, were
both 0.93. As a robustness check we ran all analyses
excluding all groups with an r
wg
below 0.80. Because the
results were equivalent to the results using the full sam-
ple, we report model output statistics from analyses with
the full sample. For HLM analyses, we specified con-
tinuous distribution outcome variables when predicting
job satisfaction and job engagement and the Bernoulli
distribution (which is appropriate for binary outcome
variables) when predicting voluntary turnover. To test
improvement of model fit, we compared deviance statis-
tics across models based on a 2-log-likelihood calcula-
tion (the higher the deviance statistic, the poorer the fit
between the model and the data). Thus, we were look-
ing for a significant reduction in the deviance statistic to
discern whether one model fits the data better than the
others. We used a Laplace deviance estimation for all
analyses involving voluntary turnover, as recommended
by Snijders and Bosker (1999, pp. 218–219). Because
our research question centers on understanding how our
level 2 predictor (leader humility) affects the propor-
tion of variance observed in each group (see Neuhaus
et al. 1991), we report the unit-specific model with
robust standard errors. To test our multilevel mediation
hypotheses for our leader humility (level 2) to job satis-
faction (level 1) to voluntary turnover (level 1) path, we
separated within- and between-group effects by group
centering the mediation variable at level 1 and reintro-
ducing the group average of the mediator at level 2, as
recommended by Zhang et al. (2009, p. 698). Because
Path 2 entails a level 2 mediator (i.e., a 2-2-1 model),
we used the grand-mean centering approach also recom-
mended by Zhang et al. (2009, p. 700). We also report
a Freedman and Schatzkin (1992) t-statistic to indicate
the significance of the mediation effect (for the equation,
see Zhang et al. 2009, p. 697).
Results. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correla-
tions for all study variables are presented in Table 7.
Leader-expressed humility was positively related to job
engagement (r = 00251 p < 0001) and job satisfaction
(r = 00441 p < 0001), and it was negatively related to vol-
untary job turnover (r = 00141 p < 0001). Job engage-
ment and job satisfaction were also negatively related to
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1531
Table 7 Study 3 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Variable Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Gender
a
0072 0045
2. Race
b
0070 0046 0008
3. Age 37045 11005 0009
0012
∗∗
4. Tenure under leader
c
12051 12075 0006
0007
0010
∗∗
5. Leader-expressed humility 3090 0089 0005 0007
0003 0005 400975
6. Job engagement 4089
d
0094 0008
0004 0019
∗∗
0010
∗∗
0025
∗∗
400925
7. Job satisfaction 3055 0071 0001 0008
0002 0006
0044
∗∗
0064
∗∗
400825
8. Team LGO
e
5001
d
1013 0001 0006 0009
0002 0035
∗∗
0046
∗∗
0052
∗∗
400925
9. Voluntary turnover
f
0011 0032 0006 0002 0004 0003 0014
∗∗
0010
∗∗
0016
∗∗
0005
Notes. N = 704 employees. Scale alpha reliabilities are given on the diagonal.
a
Represents a dummy variable: male = 0, female = 1.
b
Represents a dummy variable: non-Caucasian = 0, Caucasian = 1.
c
Represents the number of months of tenure under the current leader.
d
Scaled to a seven-point agreement scale.
e
Reflects team learning goal orientation.
f
Represents a binary variable of voluntary turnover: voluntary turnover = 1.
p < 0010;
p < 0005;
∗∗
p < 0001.
turnover (r = 00101 p < 0001 and r = 00161 p < 0001,
respectively).
Table 8 shows the results of the regression and HLM
analyses testing Hypotheses 3–5. In support of Hypoth-
esis 3, leader-expressed humility was positively related
to team LGO after controlling for demographic differ-
ences (as shown in Model 2; b = 00531 ãR
2
= 00271 p <
00001). Model 4 provides support for Hypothesis 4,
as leader-expressed humility was positively related to
job engagement after controlling for demographic dif-
ferences (
01
= 00211 p < 0001). In line with the recom-
mendations from Zhang et al. (2009) for testing multi-
level mediation effects, we added grand-mean-centered
team LGO to the equation in Model 5. The relationship
between leader-expressed humility and job engagement
decreased to nonsignificance, suggesting a full media-
tion effect of team LGO (
02
= 00301 p < 00001) and
providing support for Hypothesis 5. The Freedman and
Schatzkin (1992) t-statistic for this mediation effect was
also significant (t = 60611 p < 00001), providing addi-
tional confirmation of Hypothesis 5.
Table 9 reports the results for the tests of Hypothe-
ses 6–8. Model 2 shows support for Hypothesis 6,
as leader-expressed humility was positively related to
job satisfaction after controlling for demographic differ-
ences (
02
= 00341 p < 0001). Model 4 shows that leader-
expressed humility was negatively related to voluntary
turnover (
01
= 00821 p < 0001), supporting Hypothe-
sis 7. As shown in Model 5, this effect was mediated
by job satisfaction (
60
= 10411 p < 00001), providing
support for Hypothesis 8. The Freedman and Schatzkin
(1992) t-statistic for this mediation effect was significant
(t = 10821 p < 0005), providing additional support for
Hypothesis 8.
Discussion
There are three key contributions this study makes
that deserve mentioning. First, although several theoret-
ical and qualitative studies suggest the importance of
leader-expressed humility (Morris et al. 2005, Nielsen
et al. 2010, Owens and Hekman 2012, Vera and
Rodriguez-Lopez 2004), this is the first quantitative
study that empirically tests the benefits of leaders humil-
ity in organizational contexts. Second, this study pro-
vides important insight into the job engagement litera-
ture, which is still not definitive about what leadership
approaches best foster engaged employees. In contrast
to “rousing” employees through charismatic, energetic,
and idealistic leadership approaches (empirical evidence
has been inconsistent about these approaches; see Dvir
et al. 2002), our study suggests a “quieter” leadership
approach, with listening, being transparent about limi-
tations, and appreciating follower strengths and contri-
butions as effective ways to engage employees. Third,
this study also extends what previous research has found
regarding the impact of leadership style on retaining
employees. Companies fiercely compete to attract and
retain talented employees (Michaels et al. 2001), and
many talented employees leave firms because they feel
that their immediate leaders do not recognize their
potential or listen to them (Burke 2006). It seems, then,
that leader-expressed humility may be even more impor-
tant as firms work to attract and retain talented individu-
als. Overall, the meaning of expressed humility derived
from our literature search and initial empirical testing
appears to have important implications for employee
attitudes and organizational outcomes in the workplace.
We should also mention two limitations that are appar-
ent to us. First, we have suggested causal relationships
in our hypotheses and the direction of these causal
links. We are fairly confident that Path 2 with expressed
humility, job satisfaction, and turnover makes sense
theoretically and has some support empirically. Also, our
measurement of satisfaction was one month after mea-
suring expressed humility, and we obtained turnover four
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1532 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
Table 8 Study 3 (a) Regression and (b) HLM Analyses
Testing the Predictiveness of Leader-Expressed
Humility on Team Learning Goal Orientation (Time 1)
and Job Engagement (Time 2)
Panel (a): Regression
Variable Model 1 Model 2
Controls
a
Gender (mean)
b
0.01 0009
Race (mean)
c
0.01 0006
Tenure (mean) 0.05 0003
Tenure under leader 0.02 0000
(mean)
Predictor
Leader-expressed 0053
∗∗∗
humility
F 0.19 12096
∗∗∗
R
2
0.00 0027
ãR
2
0027
∗∗∗
Panel (b): HLM
Variable Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Intercept 4006
∗∗∗
3009
∗∗∗
4092
∗∗∗
Controls
L1: Gender
b
4
10
5 0004 0004 0004
L1: Race
c
4
20
5 0008 0008 0004
L1: Tenure (
30
5 0003
∗∗∗
0003
∗∗∗
0002
∗∗∗
L1: Tenure under 0000 0000 0000
leader (
50
5
Predictors
L2: Leader-expressed 0021
∗∗
0010
humility (
01
5
L2: Team LGO (
02
5 0030
∗∗∗
Log likelihood 11674000 11661042 11637072
26L4
reduced
full
5] 15014
∗∗
72055
∗∗∗
Notes. n = 704 employees; k = 218. For Models 1 and 2, the
dependent variable is team LGO; for Models 3–5, the dependent
variable is job engagement. All regression values reflect standard-
ized beta coefficients. L1 suggests a level 1 variable; L2 suggests
a level 2 variable.
a
All controls for regression Models 1 and 2 represent the team
mean for each variable.
b
Gender coded as 1 = male, 0 = female.
c
Race coded as 1 = white, 0 = minority.
p < 0010;
p < 0005;
∗∗
p < 0001;
∗∗∗
p < 00001.
months after measuring satisfaction. Though turnover
can most certainly be a factor leaders could be judged
on (and thus also be a cause of subsequent expressed
humility), we believe our data support the direction we
hypothesized. We are less sure about the causal direction
for Path 1. We measured both team LGO and engage-
ment at the same time. Although we have argued for
team LGO as the mediator, one could argue that engage-
ment may enhance LGO. We think our logic makes
sense, but more empirical evidence is clearly needed
here.
3
We also did not directly measure all the mediat-
ing mechanisms we included in our hypothesis rationale
(i.e., increased mutual disclosure and trust).
The other limitation has to do with the fact that we
have three dimensions of expressed humility, and in this
study (as well as Study 2) we might find differential
strength of effects for the humility dimensions and their
relationships to our dependent variables. We did not do
this for two reasons. First, we felt that our initial pre-
sentation and tests of our expressed humility construct
should include the whole set of dimensions. We did not
have the theoretical depth, previous empirical findings or
experience with our new construct to make differential
hypotheses. Second, we believe differential hypotheses
would partly be a function of the types of people in the
study, the type of industry involved, and various on-site
particulars, such as leadership training, past history, and
organizational culture. We suggest that future research
should seek to gain more nuanced insights about the
effects of humility by parsing out the unique effects of
the expressed humility dimensions.
General Discussion
To make a significant theoretical contribution, our over-
arching goals for this research were to (1) define
expressed humility in a parsimonious, theoretically novel
and meaningful way that extends beyond the current
literature; (2) develop a psychometrically robust mea-
sure of expressed humility and provide foundational
evidence for its construct, nomological, and predictive
validity; and (3) show that expressed humility is impor-
tant in organizational contexts. The three studies we have
reported represent at least five important and novel con-
tributions to the current literature. First, as the result of
our literature review, we synthesize previous conceptual-
izations of humility into one clear, operationalizable def-
inition that portrays expressed humility as a multifaceted
strength displayed in a social and interpersonal context.
This parsimonious synthesis of the expressed humility
dimensions, for which we have shown empirical support,
is an important contribution, given that some present
humility as a complex construct with up to 13 differ-
ent indicators or dimensions (Vera and Rodriguez-Lopez
2004). Though some question the concept of humil-
ity as a virtue or strength (Hume 1994, pp. 219, 270)
and some subjects still categorize humility as associated
with humiliation and lowliness (see Exline and Geyer
2004, Grenberg 2005), our research adds important evi-
dence in this debate, as our strengths-based view of
humility was supported by the positive relationships we
found between expressed humility and positive attitudes,
traits, and adaptive behaviors (i.e., self-esteem via core
self-evaluation, emotional stability, self-efficacy, perfor-
mance improvement, learning orientation, and engage-
ment). The fact that “the willingness to see oneself accu-
rately” component of expressed humility was positively
related to these positive attitudes, traits, and adaptive
behaviors supports the notion that realistic self-views
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1533
Table 9 Study 3 Hierarchical Linear Modeling Analyses Testing the Predictiveness of Leader Humility (Time 1) on Job
Satisfaction and Voluntary Turnover (Time 2)
Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
c
Model 4
c
Model 5
c
Intercept (
00
5 3057
∗∗∗
3058
∗∗∗
3056
∗∗∗
3072
∗∗∗
1002
Controls
L1: Gender
a
4
10
5 0001 0001 0085 0091 0009
L1: Race
b
4
20
5 0018
0018
∗∗
0028 0027 0006
L1: Tenure (
30
5 0000
0002
0000 0000 0001
L1: Tenure under leader (
50
5 0001
0001 0003 0003
0004
Predictor
L2: Leader-expressed humility (
01
5 0034
∗∗∗
0082
∗∗
0033
Mediators
L2: Job satisfaction group average (
02
5 1043
L1: Job satisfaction (
60
5 1041
∗∗∗
Log likelihood 11473089 11451070 11332097 11327065 11311091
26L4
reduced
full
5] 44037
∗∗∗
10064
31048
∗∗∗
Notes. n = 704 employees; k = 218. For Models 1 and 2, the dependent variable is job satisfaction; for Models 3–5, the dependent variable
is voluntary turnover. L1 suggests a level 1 variable; L2 suggests a level 2 variable.
a
Gender coded as 1 = male, 0 = female.
b
Race coded as 1 = white, 0 = minority.
c
Because voluntary turnover is a binary outcome variable, the Bernoulli function and Laplace deviance estimation (Snijders and Bosker
1999, pp. 218–219) were used when computing this model.
p < 0010;
p < 0005;
∗∗
p < 0001;
∗∗∗
p < 00001.
are more beneficial than exaggerated/inflated ones (Tay-
lor and Brown 1988, Taylor 1989, Colvin et al. 1995).
Furthermore, the empirical results of Study 1 lend
strong support for the validity and reliability of our
new expressed humility scale using an observer-report
format. Thus, our definition and operationalization of
expressed humility is a critical contribution to the humil-
ity literature, enabling empirical testing for heretofore
speculative statements about the importance of humility
in organizations.
Second, Study 2 provided evidence that expressed
humility explains unique variance in performance above
and beyond general mental ability, self-efficacy, and
conscientiousness. Intelligence taps what level a person
can perform, and conscientiousness and self-efficacy tap
what level a person is motivated to perform; expressed
humility captures a person’s receptivity to the positive
social modeling of others and responsiveness to feed-
back in taking remedial action after events of poor per-
formance (i.e., performance improvement over time).
Third, expressed humility demonstrated a compensatory
effect on performance for those with lower general men-
tal ability. Though the link between general mental abil-
ity and performance has a long history, more recently,
scholars have called for and given more attention to
other individual differences that may predict perfor-
mance (Schmidt and Hunter 2004, Poropat 2009). Mea-
sures of raw general mental ability may reflect cognitive
processing speed and the ability to grasp new concepts.
But the ability to learn is not the same thing as a will-
ingness to learn. General mental ability says little about
how willing someone is to seek and apply critical feed-
back, admit mistakes, and benefit from the positive mod-
eling of others. Humble people with lower general men-
tal ability may still perform well because they may be
more willing to enact these learning behaviors to master
performance tasks.
Fourth, given the general trend of organizing around
teams, constructs such as expressed humility may make
unique contributions to explain aspects of an individual’s
performance at work, above and beyond general men-
tal ability. How one responds to the increased frequency
and intensity of association with others may be impor-
tant for understanding who thrives and does not thrive
in team-based environments. As we showed in Study 2,
expressed humility was positively related to team contri-
bution ratings. Thus, the tendencies of feedback recep-
tivity, enhanced social learning, and having realistic
views of oneself may be particularly beneficial for indi-
vidual performance improvement and effectiveness in
these team-based contexts. Finally, Study 3 provides evi-
dence that expressed humility is an important component
of effective leadership in modern organizations. In this
study we showed that humble leaders foster learning-
oriented teams and engaged employees as well as job
satisfaction and employee retention. Showing external
and predictive validity for leader-expressed humility
construct enables the existing propositions about hum-
ble leadership to be empirically tested (see Morris et al.
2005, Nielsen et al. 2010, Owens and Hekman 2012).
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
Although our work clearly makes some important con-
tributions, there are also some limitations. First, the
degree to which the findings of Study 2 generalize to
the workplace is uncertain, given the student sample and
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1534 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
the performance criteria. However, the experience upon
which the expressed humility ratings were based—team
member interactions in longitudinal project teams—were
designed to simulate a realistic work context. Thus,
though our objective measure of individual performance
was clearly academic, the measuring of expressed humil-
ity (our main theoretical focus) by members of a long-
standing project team provides a naturalistic setting
that closely approximates a real work-team context. We
also felt, given the implications of expressed humil-
ity on learning, that exploring the connection between
expressed humility and performance in an educational
context was a good theoretical “fit” with conducting ini-
tial explorations of this new construct. Testing whether
expressed humility explained variance beyond mental
ability in an academic environment seemed more con-
servative, because this is the environment where mental
ability has historically been the most predictive of per-
formance (compared with the work context; see Murphy
1996). Though our hypothesized relationships were con-
firmed in this educational setting, we encourage future
research to replicate the relationships between expressed
humility and performance and the expressed humility
compensatory effect in a work setting.
Second, the results from Study 3 may be inflated
because we used a common source of data for ratings
of leader-expressed humility, follower job satisfaction,
job engagement, and team learning orientation. How-
ever, we sought to minimize common method variance
by temporally spacing the measurement of predictor and
criterion variables and by using different referents (i.e.,
leader, self, team) for rating the study constructs (Pod-
sakoff et al. 2003). There were limitations in each study,
but we propose that the strengths of each help compen-
sate for the weaknesses of the others (see Wiesenfeld
et al. 2007, p. 1250). As a set, we suggest that the eight
samples used across our three studies provide convinc-
ing support that expressed humility is a unique, valid,
and predictive construct that deserves more attention in
organizational research.
Third, although our studies provide a foundation for
what expressed humility is, what it is associated with,
and what it influences, they say little about what indi-
vidual or situational factors foster the expression of
humility in the workplace. Future research should seek
to more fully understand the antecedents of expressed
humility. Such understanding will help better inform
organizations on how to select for or develop this
attribute in employees and leaders. Exline et al. (2004)
suggest that secure relational attachments, reality-based
feedback about one’s strengths and weaknesses, and
not having extreme emphasis placed on performance in
one’s past school (and perhaps work) experience may
be antecedents of personal humility. Future research
should also examine how relative power, status, and
other demographic characteristics influence the expres-
sion of humility (Owens and Hekman 2012).
We also see the need for more research on the effects
of expressed humility. We suggest that our peer-report
scale may allow researchers to address questions such
as those proposed by Tangney (2000), including to what
degree expressed humility might be related to dimen-
sions of psychological and physical health, the devel-
opment of other virtues (e.g., forgiveness, altruism), its
connection to common cognitive biases (e.g., overconfi-
dence or hindsight bias) and emotions (e.g., pride, envy),
and the degree to which expressed humility may help or
hinder occupational success in a competitive world. We
also encourage future research to explore team member-
expressed humility in team contexts and, specifically,
to examine whether the interactive effects of expressed
humility and intelligence on team member contribu-
tions, process, and outcomes may vary across different
types of team tasks (i.e., disjunctive, conjunctive; see
Steiner 1972).
In addition, future research should further explore
the relevance and benefits of expressed humility in
the context of leadership. Leadership scholars should
empirically examine whether leader-expressed humility
does in fact foster greater learning, adaptiveness, and
extraordinary performance (see Collins 2005, Vera and
Rodriguez-Lopez 2004) or whether it means the differ-
ence between true and “pseudo”-transformational lead-
ership (see Morris et al. 2005), propositions that have
not yet been empirically tested. Future research should
also examine boundary conditions for leader-expressed
humility. For example, the level of leadership (first-line,
middle management, executive management) or culture
(Western versus Eastern) may influence the effectiveness
of leader-expressed humility.
We also propose that future research test how ex-
pressed humility might interact with the closely related
constructs presented in Table 1. We expect, for exam-
ple, that expressed humility may have a tempering or
buffering influence on LGO, CSE, and openness to
experience that will enhance the utility of these con-
structs within organizational contexts. Driven by intrin-
sic curiosity (openness to experience), favorable views
toward task persistence (LGO), or an unrealistically high
sense of control over outcomes (internal locus of con-
trol), these otherwise positive characteristics could lead
to undue persistence in failing causes or in tasks one has
little chance of accomplishing. Expressed humility may
help buffer extreme or maladaptive levels of these con-
structs to avoid such ineffective path persistence. That is,
a willingness to view oneself accurately (including limi-
tations) and an openness to receiving feedback from oth-
ers may help temper these characteristics, adding more
accuracy to self-efficacy, more realism to perceptions of
control, and more purpose or grounding to openness to
experience. Thus, we recommend that future research
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1535
examine interactions between expressed humility and
other predictors of individual and team-level outcomes.
One final point is especially important to make. The
recent movements of positive psychology (Seligman
and Csikszentmihalyi 2000) and positive organizational
scholarship (Cameron et al. 2003) have spurred more
attention and interest in virtues such as humility. How-
ever, studying virtues has a unique set of challenges.
First, compared with many psychological constructs,
virtues typically have rich historic roots in theologi-
cal and philosophical literatures, often making it more
difficult to gain consensus about how to operational-
ize these virtues. Additionally, because virtues generally
have greater moral underpinnings than many personal-
ity traits, accessing these constructs through self-report
becomes more challenging (see Allison et al. 1989). We
hope that our work can serve as a template for further
research on virtues within organizational contexts.
Conclusion
The scientific study of humility within organizations
promises many exciting implications and new avenues of
research. Tangney (2000, p. 80) suggested that the study
of humility would be propelled forward significantly
with “a clear and consistently articulated conceptualiza-
tion of this rich construct and, in turn, by the develop-
ment of theoretically informed measures of humility.
Although scholars have argued for the importance of
humility within leadership and organizational contexts,
this research topic has been stymied because of a lack of
consensus with definitions and the difficulty of measure-
ment. We hope that the development and validation of
this humility scale represent an important step forward
in being able to examine the real impact of humility on
important organizational processes and outcomes, and
that this effort will spur more theory and research involv-
ing this “classical source of strength” (Tangney 2000,
p. 70) within organizations.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Batia Wiesenfeld and three anonymous
reviewers for their helpful feedback on this project. The
authors are also grateful to Scott Reynolds and Will Felps for
their encourageent and support during the early stages of this
research. This article was supported in part by a grant from
the John Templeton Foundation [Grant 29630] entitled “The
Development, Validation, and Dissemination of Measures of
Intellectual Humility and Humility.
Endnotes
1
The constructs of modesty, narcissism, openness to experi-
ence, and honesty-humility were the most often mentioned
in association with humility in our review of the literature.
From our reading about how humility might fit into the orga-
nizational literature, we identified learning goal orientation
and core self-evaluations as germane constructs with which to
compare expressed humility.
2
For example, “When my leader shows humility and is open to
what others have to say it creates an environment of energy”
and humble leaders “play to their [employees’] strengths
and 0 0 0 create a passion that excites them about what they do”
(Owens and Hekman 2012, p. 804).
3
As a robustness check, we sought to assess this reverse
mediation possibility. Because HLM requires a level 1 out-
come, we conducted this robustness check with individual
(level 1) perceptions of team LGO. Replicating the multilevel
effects, we found that individual perceptions of team LGO
mediated the relationship between leader-expressed humil-
ity and individuals’ reported engagement; we found no evi-
dence that individuals’ engagement mediated the relationship
between leader-expressed humility and individual perceptions
of team LGO.
References
Allison ST, Messick DM, Goethals GR (1989) On being better but not
smarter than others: The Muhammad Ali effect. Soc. Cognition
7(3):275–295.
Ames C (1992) Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation.
J. Ed. Psych. 84(3):261–271.
Anderson C, Srivastava S, Beer JS, Spataro SE, Chatman JA (2006)
Knowing your place: Self-perceptions of status in face-to-face
groups. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 91(6):1094–1110.
Anderson JC, Gerbing DW (1991) Predicting the performance of mea-
sures in a confirmatory factor analysis with a pretest assessment
of their substantive validities. J. Appl. Psych. 76(5):732–740.
Ashton MC, Lee K (2008) The prediction of honesty-humility-related
criteria by the HEXACO and five-factor models of personality.
J. Res. Personality 42(5):1216–1228.
Avolio BJ, Walumbwa FO, Weber TJ (2009) Leadership: Current
theories, research, and future directions. Annual Rev. Psych.
60:421–449.
Bales RF (1950) Interaction Process Analysis (Addison-Wesley,
Cambridge, MA).
Barrick MR, Mount MK (1991) The Big Five personality dimen-
sions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personality Psych.
44(1):1–26.
Baumeister RF, Jones EE (1978) When self-presentation is con-
strained by the target’s knowledge: Consistency and compensa-
tion. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 36(6):608–618.
Boje DM, Rosile GA, Durant RA, Luhman JT (2004) Enron
spectacles: A critical dramaturgical analysis. Organ. Stud.
25(5):751–774.
Bollen K, Lennox R (1991) Conventional wisdom on measurement:
A structural equation perspective. Psych. Bull. 110(2):305–314.
Boswell WR, Boudreau JW, Tichy J (2005) The relationship between
employee job change and job satisfaction: The honeymoon-
hangover effect. J. Appl. Psych. 90(5):882–892.
Bunderson JS, Sutcliffe KM (2003) Management team learning
orientation and business unit performance. J. Appl. Psych.
88(3):552–560.
Burke RJ (2006) Why leaders fail: Exploring the darkside. Internat.
J. Manpower 27(1):91–100.
Cameron KS, Dutton JE, Quinn RE (2003) Positive Organizational
Scholarship: Foundations of a New Discipline (Berrett-Koehler,
San Francisco).
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1536 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
Church A, Tornow W, Burke W, Hogan R, Avolio B (1998) From
both sides now: Leadership—So close and yet so far. Indust.
Organ. Psychologist 35(3):1–14.
Collins J (2005) Level 5 leadership: The triumph of humility and
fierce resolve. Harvard Bus. Rev. 79(1):66–76.
Collins NL, Miller LC (1994) Self-disclosure and liking: A meta-
analytic review. Psych. Bull. 116(3):457–475.
Colquitt JA (2008) From the editors: Publishing laboratory research
in AMJ: A question of when, not if. Acad. Management J.
51(4):616–620.
Colvin CR, Block J, Funder DC (1995) Overly positive self-
evaluations and personality: Negative implications for mental
health. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 68(6):1152–1162.
Cook JD, Hepworth SJ, Wall TD, Warr PB (1981) The Experience of
Work: A Compendium and Review of 249 Measures and Their
Use (Academic Press, London).
Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992) Normal personality assessment in clin-
ical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psych. Assess.
4(1):5–13.
Côté S, Miners CTH (2006) Emotional intelligence, cognitive intelli-
gence, and job performance. Admin. Sci. Quart. 51(1):1–28.
Cropanzano R, Bowen DE, Gilliland SW (2007) The manage-
ment of organizational justice. Acad. Management Perspect.
21(4):34–48.
Dane E, Pratt MG (2007) Exploring intuition and its role in manage-
rial decision making. Acad. Management Rev. 32(1):33–54.
Davis DE, Worthington EL Jr, Hook JN (2010) Humility: Review
of measurement strategies and conceptualization as personality
judgment. J. Positive Psych. 5(4):243–252.
DeVellis RF (2011) Scale Development: Theory and Applications, 3rd
ed. (Sage, Newbury Park, CA).
Dickter DN, Roznowski M, Harrison DA (1996) Temporal tempering:
An event history analysis of the process of voluntary turnover.
J. Appl. Psych. 81(6):705–716.
Dirks KT, Ferrin DL (2002) Trust in leadership: Meta-analytic find-
ings and implications for research and practice. J. Appl. Psych.
87(4): 611–628.
Dodrill CB (1983) Long-term reliability of the Wonderlic Personnel
Test. J. Consulting Clinical Psych. 51(2):316–317.
Dotlich DL, Cairo PC (2003) Why CEOs Fail: The 11 Behaviors That
Can Derail Your Climb to the Top—And How to Manage Them
(Jossey-Bass, San Francisco).
Dvir T, Eden D, Avolio BJ, Shamir B (2002) Impact of transfor-
mational leadership on follower development and performance:
A field experiment. Acad. Management J. 45(4):735–744.
Dweck CS (2000) Self-Theories: Their Role in Motivation, Personal-
ity, and Development (Psychology Press, Philadelphia).
Dweck CS, Leggett EL (1988) A social-cognitive approach to moti-
vation and personality. Psych. Rev. 95(2):256–273.
Edmondson, A (1999) Psychological safety and learning behavior in
work teams. Admin. Sci. Quart. 44(2):350–383.
Edwards JR, Bagozzi RP (2000) On the nature and direction of
relationships between constructs and measures. Psych. Methods
5(2):155–174.
Ehrlich HJ, Graeven DB (1971) Reciprocal self-disclosure in a dyad.
J. Experiment. Soc. Psych. 7(4):389–400.
Exline JJ, Geyer A (2004) Perceptions of humility: A preliminary
study. Self Identity 3(2):95–114.
Exline JJ, Campbell WK, Baumeister RF, Joiner T, Krueger J,
Kachorek LV (2004) Humility and modesty. Peterson C,
Seligman M, eds. Character Strengths and Virtues: A Hand-
book and Classification (Values in Action Institute, Cincinnati),
461–475.
Ferris GR (1985) Role of leadership in the employee withdrawal pro-
cess: A constructive replication. J. Appl. Psych. 70(4):777–781.
Finkelstein S (2003) Why Smart Executives Fail: And What You Can
Learn from Their Mistakes (Portfolio Trade, New York).
Foa UG, Foa EB (1974) Societal Structures of the Mind (Charles C.
Thomas, Oxford, UK).
Freedman LS, Schatzkin A (1992) Sample size for studying interme-
diate endpoints within intervention trials of observational stud-
ies. Amer. J. Epidemiol. 136(9):1148–1159.
Fulmer RM, Conger JA (2004) Growing Your Company’s Leaders:
How Great Organizations Use Succession Management to Sus-
tain Competitive Advantage (AMACOM, New York).
Furnham A, Hosoe T, Tang TLP (2002) Male hubris and female
humility? A crosscultural study of ratings of self, parental, and
sibling multiple intelligence in America, Britain, and Japan.
Intelligence 30(1):101–115.
Gong Y, Huang JC, Farh JL (2009) Employee learning orientation,
transformational leadership, and employee creativity: The medi-
ating role of employee creative self-efficacy. Acad. Management
J. 52(4):765–778.
Gosling SD, Ko SJ, Mannarelli T, Morris ME (2002) A room with
a cue: Personality judgments based on offices and bedrooms.
J. Personality Soc. Psych. 82(3):379–398.
Graen GB, Liden RC, Hoel W (1982) Role of leadership in the
employee withdrawal process. J. Appl. Psych. 67(6):868–872.
Grenberg JM (2005) Précis of Kant and the Ethics of Humility:
A Story of Dependence, Corruption and Virtue (Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge, MA).
Griffeth RW, Hom PW, Gaertner S (2000) A meta-analysis of
antecedents and correlates of employee turnover: Update, mod-
erator tests, and research implications for the next millennium.
J. Management 26(3):463–488.
Hareli S, Weiner B (2000) Accounts for success as determi-
nants of perceived arrogance and modesty. Motiv. Emotion
24(3):215–236.
Hiller NJ, Hambrick DC (2005) Conceptualizing executive hubris:
The role of (hyper-)core self-evaluations in strategic decision-
making. Strategic Management J. 26(4):297–319.
Hinkin TR (1995) A review of scale development practices in the
study of organizations. J. Management 21(5):967–988.
Hogan R, Kaiser RB (2005) What we know about leadership. Rev.
General Psych. 9(2):169–180.
Holtom BC, Mitchell TR, Lee TW, Bussmann MB (2008) Turnover
and retention research: A glance at the past, a closer review of
the present, and a venture into the future. Acad. Management
Ann. 2(1):231–274.
Hom PW, Griffeth RW (1995) Employee Turnover (South-Western,
Cincinnati).
Horowitz LM, Wilson KR, Turan B, Zolotsev P, Constantino MJ,
Henderson L (2006) How interpersonal motives clarify the
meaning of interpersonal behavior: A revised circumplex model.
Personality Soc. Psych. Rev. 10(1):67–86.
Hume D (1994) Political Writings (Hackett Publishing, Indianapolis).
Jackson DN (1977) Reliability of the Jackson personality inventory.
Psych. Rep. 40(2):613–614.
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS 1537
John OP, Robins RW (1993) Determinants of interjudge agreement
on personality traits: The Big Five domains, observability, eval-
uativeness, and the unique perspective of the self. J. Personality
61(4):521–551.
Judge TA, Erez A, Bono JE, Thoresen CJ (2003) The core self-
evaluations scale: Development of a measure. Personality Psych.
56(2):303–331.
Kahn WA (1990) Psychological conditions of personal engage-
ment and disengagement at work. Acad. Management J.
33(4):692–724.
Kenny DA (1994) Interpersonal Perception: A Social Relations Anal-
ysis (Guilford Press, New York).
King LA, Hicks JA (2007) Whatever happened to “what might
have been”? Regrets, happiness, and maturity. Amer. Psych.
62(7):625–636.
Kipnis D (1972) Does power corrupt? J. Personality Soc. Psych.
24(1):33–41.
Kline RB (1998) Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Mod-
eling (Guilford Press, New York).
Knight PA, Nadel JI (1986) Humility revisited: Self-esteem, infor-
mation search, and policy consistency. Organ. Behav. Human
Decision Processes 38(2):196–206.
Kolar DW, Funder DC, Colvin CR (1996) Comparing the accuracy
of personality judgments by the self and knowledgeable others.
J. Personality 64(2):311–337.
Lawrence PR, Nohria N (2002) Driven: How Human Nature Shapes
Our Choices (Jossey-Bass, San Francisco).
Lubit R (2002) The long-term organizational impact of destruc-
tively narcissistic managers. Acad. Management Executive
16(1):127–138.
March JG, Simon HA (1958) Organizations (John Wiley & Sons,
Oxford, UK).
Margolis HD, Thomas V (1980) The measurement of narcissism
in adolescents with and without behavioral and emotional dis-
abilities Master’s thesis, United States International University,
San Diego.
McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr (1987) Validation of the five-factor model
of personality across instruments and observers. J. Personality
Soc. Psych. 52(1):81–90.
McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr (1989) The structure of interpersonal traits:
Wiggin’s circumplex and the five-factor model. J. Personality
Soc. Psych. 56(4):586–595.
McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr (2004) A contemplated revision of the
NEO five-factor inventory. Personality Individual Differences
36(3):587–596.
McIntyre RM, Salas E (1995) Measuring and managing for team
performance: Emerging principles from complex environments.
Guzzom A, Salas E, eds. Team Effectiveness and Decision Mak-
ing in Organizations (Jossey-Bass, San Francisco), 9–45.
Means JR, Wilson GL, Sturm C, Biron JE, Bach PJ (1990) The-
ory and practice: Humility as a psychotherapeutic formulation.
Counseling Quart. 3(2):211–215.
Meece JL, Blumenfeld PC, Hoyle RH (1988) Students’ goal orien-
tations and cognitive engagement in classroom activities. J. Ed.
Psych. 80(4):514–523.
Michaels E, Handfield-Jones H, Axelrod B (2001) The War for Talent
(Harvard Business Press, Boston).
Mischel W, Shoda Y (1995) A cognitive-affective system the-
ory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions,
dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psych. Rev.
102(2):246–268.
Morris JA, Brotheridge CM, Urbanski JC (2005) Bringing humility
to leadership: Antecedents and consequences of leader humility.
Human Relations 58(10):1323–1350.
Murphy KR (1996) Individual differences and behavior in orga-
nizations: Much more than g. Murphy KR, ed. Individ-
ual Differences and Behavior in Organizations (Jossey-Bass,
San Francisco), 3–30.
Neuhaus JM, Kalbfleisch JD, Hauck WW (1991) A comparison of
cluster-specific and population-averaged approaches for analyz-
ing correlated binary data. Internat. Statist. Rev. 59:25–35.
Nielsen R, Marrone JA, Slay HS (2010) A new look at humility:
Exploring the humility concept and its role in socialized charis-
matic leadership. J. Leadership Organ. Stud. 17(1):33–43.
Owens BP (2011) Humility in Organizational Leadership (Pro-
Quest/UMI Dissertation Publishing, Ann Arbor, MI).
Owens BP, Hekman DR (2012) Modeling how to grow: An induc-
tive examination of humble leader behaviors, contingencies, and
outcomes. Acad. Management J. 55(4):787–818.
Park N, Peterson CM (2003) Virtues and organizations. Cameron
KS, Dutton JE, Quinn RE, eds. Positive Organizational Schol-
arship: Foundations of a New Discipline (Berret-Koehler,
San Francisco), 33–47.
Perry SJ, Witt LA, Penney LM, Atwater L (2010) The downside of
goal-focused leadership: The role of personality in subordinate
exhaustion. J. Appl. Psych. 95(6):1145–1153.
Peterson C, Seligman M (2004) The Values in Action VIA Classifica-
tion of Strengths (Values in Action Institute, Cincinnati).
Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP (2003) Com-
mon method biases in behavioral research: A critical review
of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psych.
88(5):879–903.
Poropat AE (2009) A meta-analysis of the Five-Factor Model of Per-
sonality on academic performance. Psych. Bull. 135(2):322–338.
Rich BL, Lepine JA, Crawford ER (2010) Job engagement:
Antecedents and effects on job performance. Acad. Management
J. 53(3):617–635.
Richards N (1992) Humility (Temple University Press, Philadelphia).
Richardson HA, Vandenberg RJ (2005) Integrating managerial per-
ceptions and transformational leadership into a work unit
level model of employee involvement. J. Organ. Behavior
26(5):561–589.
Roberts RC, Wood WJ (2003) Humility and epistemic goods. Intel-
lectual Virtue 1(9):257–298.
Rosenthal SA, Pittinsky TL (2006) Narcissistic leadership. Leadership
Quart. 17(6):617–633.
Russell SS, Spitzmüller C, Lin LF, Stanton JM, Smith PC,
Ironson GH (2004) Shorter can also be better: The abridged job
in general scale. Ed. Psych. Measurement 64(5):878–893.
Schaufeli WB, Bakker AB (2004) Job demands, job resources, and
their relationship with burnout and engagement: A multi sample
study. J. Organ. Behav. 25(3):293–315.
Schaufeli WB, Salanova M, González-Romá V, Bakker AB (2002)
The measurement of engagement and burnout: A two sam-
ple confirmatory factor analytic approach. J. Happiness Stud.
3(1):71–92.
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.
Owens, Johnson, and Mitchell: Expressed Humility in Organizations
1538 Organization Science 24(5), pp. 1517–1538, © 2013 INFORMS
Schmidt FL, Hunter J (2004) General mental ability in the world of
work: Occupational attainment and job performance. J. Person-
ality Soc. Psych. 86(1):162–173.
Schwab DP (1980) Construct validity in organization behavior.
Staw BM, Cummings LL, eds. Research in Organizational
Behavior, Vol. 2 (JAI Press, Greenwich, CT), 3–43.
Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M (1995) Generalized self-efficacy scale.
Weinman J, Wright S, Johnston M, eds. Measures in Health Psy-
chology: A User’s Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs (Nfer-
Nelson, Windsor, UK), 35–37.
Seligman MEP, Csikszentmihalyi M (2000) Positive psychology: An
introduction. Amer. Psych. 55(1):5–14.
Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ (1999) Multilevel Analysis: An Intro-
duction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Modeling (Sage,
Thousand Oaks, CA).
Steiner ID (1972) Group Process and Productivity (Academic Press,
New York).
Sullivan HS (1953) The Interpersonal Theory of Psychiatry (W. W.
Norton & Company, New York).
Tangney JP (2000) Humility: Theoretical perspectives, empirical find-
ings and directions for future research. J. Soc. Clinical Psych.
19(1):70–82.
Tangney JP (2002) Humility. Snyder CR, Lopez SJ, eds. Handbook
of Positive Psychology (Oxford University Press, New York),
411–422.
Taylor SE (1989) Positive Illusions: Creative Self-Deception and the
Healthy Mind (Basic Books, New York).
Taylor SE, Brown JD (1988) Illusion and well-being: A social
psychological perspective on mental health. Psych. Bull.
103(2):193–210.
Templeton JM (1997) Worldwide Laws of Life (Templeton Foundation
Press, Philadelphia).
Tichy NM, Cohen EB (1997) The Leadership Engine: How Win-
ning Companies Build Leaders at Every Level (Harper Collins,
New York).
Turner JC, Midgley C, Meyer DK, Gheen M, Anderman EM, Kang Y,
Patrick H (2002) The classroom environment and students’
reports of avoidance strategies in mathematics: A multimethod
study. J. Ed. Psych. 94(1):88–106.
Ungerer JA, Waters B, Barnett B, Dolby R (1997) Defense style
and adjustment in interpersonal relationships. J. Res. Personality
31(3):375–384.
Vaillant G (1992) The Wisdom of the Ego (Harvard University Press,
Cambridge, MA).
Vandewalle D (1997) Development and validation of a work domain
goal orientation instrument. Ed. Psych. Measurement 57(6):
995–1015.
Vera D, Rodriguez-Lopez A (2004) Humility as a source of compet-
itive advantage. Organ. Dynamics 33(4):393–408.
Weick KE (2001) Leadership as the legitimation of doubt. Bennis W,
Spreitzer GM, Cummings TG, eds. The Future of Leadership:
Today’s Top Leadership Thinkers Speak to Tomorrow’s Leaders
(Jossey-Bass, San Francisco), 91–102.
Wiesenfeld BM, Swann WB, Brockner J, Bartel CA (2007) Is more
fairness always preferred? Self-esteem moderates reactions to
procedural justice. Acad. Management J. 50(5):1235–1253.
Wiggins JS (1979) A psychological taxonomy of trait-descriptive
terms: The interpersonal domain. J. Personality Soc. Psych.
37(3):395–412.
Wonderlic EF (1973) Wonderlic Personnel Test: Manual (Western
Psychological Services, Los Angeles).
Zahra SA, George G (2002) Absorptive capacity: A review, recon-
ceptualization, and extension. Acad. Management Rev. 27(2):
185–203.
Zhang Z, Zyphur MJ, Preacher KJ (2009) Testing multilevel medi-
ation using hierarchical linear models: Problems and solutions.
Organ. Res. Methods 12(4):695–719.
Bradley P. Owens is an assistant professor in the School of
Management at the State University of New York at Buffalo.
He holds a Ph.D. in organizational behavior from the Univer-
sity of Washington. His current research focuses on leadership,
identity, team processes, diversity, and work/life balance.
Michael D. Johnson is an associate professor of manage-
ment and organization in the Foster School of Business at
the University of Washington. He received his Ph.D. in orga-
nizational behavior and human resource management from
Michigan State University. His research focuses primarily on
groups and teams, identity, and affect.
Terence R. Mitchell received a bachelor’s degree from
Duke and an M.A. and Ph.D. from the University of Illinois in
organizational psychology. With the University of Washington
since 1969, he has published on the topics of motivation,
leadership, turnover, and decision making. A fellow of the
Academy of Management (AoM) and the Society for Indus-
trial and Organizational Psychology (SIOP), he has received
the Lifetime Achievement Award from both SIOP and the
AoM’s Organizational Behavior division.
CORRECTION
In this article, “Expressed Humility in Organizations: Implications for Performance, Teams, and Leadership”
by Bradley P. Owens, Michael D. Johnson, and Terence R. Mitchell (first published in Articles in Advance,
February 12, 2013, Organization Science, DOI:10.1287/orsc.1120.0795), we have corrected the text so that
“Hypothesis 3” appears in lines 4 and 5 in the left-hand column of page 1531.
Downloaded from informs.org by [108.179.189.54] on 25 October 2013, at 09:49 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.