http://jlo.sagepub.com/
Organizational Studies
Journal of Leadership &
http://jlo.sagepub.com/content/20/1/62
The online version of this article can be found at:
DOI: 10.1177/1548051812467208
2013 20: 62 originally published online 27 November 2012Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies
Robert L. Porter and Gary P. Latham
The Effect of Employee Learning Goals and Goal Commitment on Departmental Performance
Published by:
http://www.sagepublications.com
On behalf of:
Midwest Academy of Management
can be found at:Journal of Leadership & Organizational StudiesAdditional services and information for
http://jlo.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:
http://jlo.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:
http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:
What is This?
- Nov 27, 2012OnlineFirst Version of Record
- Feb 5, 2013Version of Record >>
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Journal of Leadership &
Organizational Studies
20(1) 62 –68
© Baker College 2013
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1548051812467208
http://jlo.sagepub.com
In his review of the literature, Yukl (2012) noted that
“leadership is the process of influencing others to under-
stand and agree about what needs to be done and how to
do it, and the process of facilitating individual collective
effort to accomplish shared objectives” (p. 7). Thus,
among the most important tasks for leaders is motivating
and guiding the people who report to them. This is particu-
larly true during environmental and organizational uncer-
tainty when company resources may be even scarcer than
during times of relative stability. In an uncertain environ-
ment, setting appropriate employee goals and motivating
employees to commit to these goals is likely to be more
important than during times of resource abundance (Latham,
2004). Consequently, in this study, the relationship of three
types of goals, and employee commitment to these different
types of goals with department performance was examined.
No previous study has examined the relationship of these
three goals to performance in organizational settings.
Achieving departmental performance expectations in
times of uncertainty may require leaders and their employ-
ees to adapt in response to emerging threats or opportunities.
Yukl and Mahsud (2010) argued that organizational adapta-
tion typically involves a change in goals and the redirection
of key human resources. The role the leader plays in choos-
ing these goals and motivating employees to commit to these
goals may be one of the key adaptations required of a leader.
Motivating employees to pursue goals, whether the goals are
new or existing, has been shown to be a fundamental require-
ment in realizing desired performance outcomes.
Goal-Setting Theory
Locke and Latham’s (1990, 2002) goal-setting theory states
that a specific high goal leads to higher performance than urg-
ing people to do their best. Three of the four mediators are
primarily motivational, namely choice, effort, and persis-
tence. A fourth mediator is primarily cognitive. A goal cues an
individual to recall extant knowledge/skills necessary to
attain the goal. More than 1,000 studies in laboratory and
field settings, involving myriad tasks, performed by indi-
viduals as well as groups have provided empirical support
for this aspect of the theory (Mitchell & Daniels, 2003).
This is because a specific goal is a regulatory mechanism
for individuals to monitor, evaluate, and adjust their
behavior. Moreover, a specific goal often provides a
“strong situation” as to requisite behavior (Mischel, 1968).
Because the primary focus of goal-setting theory is
motivation, the tasks used in testing it are typically those
that an individual or group have already mastered. There is
little or no uncertainty or ambiguity on how to perform
them (Locke & Latham, 1990). With few exceptions, the
467208JLO20110.1177/1548051812467208Journal
of Leadership & Organizational StudiesPorter and Latham
© Baker College 2013
Reprints and permission: http://www.
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
1
University of Central Florida, Orlando, FL, USA
2
University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Corresponding Author:
Robert L. Porter, University of Central Florida, College of Business,
Management Department, 4000 Central Florida Boulevard, Orlando,
Florida, 32816-1400, USA
The Effect of Employee Learning Goals
and Goal Commitment on Departmental
Performance
Robert L. Porter
1
and Gary P. Latham
2
Abstract
The relationship between employee goals at the individual level and firm performance at the department level was examined
across a variety of industries. Specifically, three types of employee goals—learning, performance, and do-your-best—were
studied with regard to department-level performance. Employee learning goals were related to higher levels of department-
level performance than were performance or do-your-best goals. The relationship between the level of employee goal
commitment and department level performance was also examined, and found to be positive and significant. The theoretical
and practical significance of these findings for leaders in an economically turbulent environment are discussed.
Keywords
learning goals, performance goals, goal commitment, department-level performance
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Porter and Latham 63
goals studied by behavioral scientists have been specific
performance outcomes to be attained. Several laboratory
experiments, however, have shown that when people lack
the knowledge/skill to perform at a specified high level, set-
ting a vague rather than a specific goal, namely urging them
to do their best, results in higher performance than specify-
ing a high outcome to be attained (e.g., Kanfer & Ackerman,
1989; Winters & Latham, 1996). This is because a specific
high performance goal imposes greater attentional demands
on people when they are in a learning mode than is the case
when people are given a vague or abstract goal, such as to
“do-your-best.” Effective performance on a task that is
complex for an individual requires, in addition to effort, the
discovery of the appropriate strategies for performing effec-
tively. Mone and Shalley (1995), using a task where people
lacked the knowledge to perform it, found that the dysfunc-
tional effect of a specific high performance goal increased
over a 3-day period while the performance of those with a
“do-your-best” goal became increasingly better. Rather
than searching systematically for effective strategies, those
with a specific high performance goal to attain appeared to
be mindlessly switching from one strategy to another in
order to attain it.
Wood and Locke (1990) found that the effect size of
goals is usually smaller on complex than simple tasks. The
beneficial effects of goal setting, they said, are often delayed
on complex tasks because learning is required. Hence,
Winters and Latham (1996) hypothesized that a high perfor-
mance outcome goal should be set only when the person or
group has the ability to perform the task effectively. Ability
is a moderator variable in goal-setting theory (Latham &
Locke, 2007; Locke & Latham, 2002).
Of the 1,000 or more studies on goal setting, only 8 have
been conducted on the effect of a learning goal on perfor-
mance (Kaplan, Erez, & Van-Dijk, 2004; Kozlowski &
Bell, 2006; Latham & Brown, 2006; Latham, Seijts, &
Crim, 2008; Noel & Latham, 2006; Seijts & Latham, 2001;
Seijts, Latham, Tasa, & Latham, 2004; Winters & Latham,
1996). A learning goal enables a leader to focus employees’
attention on acquiring the knowledge/ability for performing
a task effectively rather than relying on the knowledge/skill
employees already possess (Seijts & Latham, 2005).
Consistent with empirical research on performance goals
(Locke & Latham, 1990), research on learning goals has
been proceeding inductively. Winters and Latham (1996)
began research on learning goals by addressing two ques-
tions: Does a learning goal, as is the case for a performance
goal, have a positive effect on subsequent task performance?
Does the type of task, where a person has/has not the ability
to perform effectively moderate the effect of both learning
and performance goals? The answer to both questions was
shown in their study to be yes. A performance goal only
increases task performance when a person has the requisite
knowledge/ability. When ability is lacking, a vague goal
increases task performance relative to a performance goal.
But, a specific high-learning goal increases task perfor-
mance significantly more than either a do-your-best or per-
formance goal. This is because a learning goal draws
attention away from the end result and emphasizes the
importance of understanding the task. The focus is on dis-
covering/developing a plan for performing it effectively
(Seijts & Latham, 2005). This finding has been replicated in
laboratory settings by Drach-Zahavy and Erez (2002) and
Kozlowski and Bell (2006). Latham et al. (2008) found that,
as is the case with a performance goal, the higher the learn-
ing goal the higher a person’s performance.
In an educational setting, Latham and Brown (2006)
found that first-year MBA students who self-set a specific
high learning goal regarding ways to make their education
meaningful to them had a significantly higher grade point
average at the end of the academic year than those who
either set a specific high distal performance goal or were
urged by the Dean to “do-your-best” to obtain a meaningful
education. Furthermore, satisfaction with the MBA pro-
gram was highest for those people in the learning goal
condition.
Model and Hypotheses
The hypothesized model that was tested in this study is
shown in Figure 1. A major limitation of the paucity of
studies of learning goals is that the majority of them have
been conducted in a laboratory (e.g., Latham et al., 2008),
a simulation (e.g., Seijts et al., 2004), or an educational set-
ting (Latham & Brown, 2006). Thus, the generalizability of
these findings to organizational settings has yet to be exam-
ined. The present study is based on field data that spanned
a variety of industries, and hence addresses this limitation.
The environment in which this study was conducted was
one of high economic and employment uncertainty. As the
Financial Times (Freeland, 2009) noted, the global eco-
nomic crisis had bankrupted century-old institutions and
brought down once-mighty industrial organizations (e.g.,
AIG, General Motors, Lehman Brothers). Unemployment in
the United States was relatively high. Hence, it was hypoth-
esized that a leaders choice of goal type has a significant
Employee Goal Type:
Learning
Performance
Do-your-best
Department
Performance as
Perceived by
Supervisor
H1+
Employee Goal
Commitment
H2+
Figure 1. Theoretical model
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from
64 Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies 20(1)
effect on a department’s performance. As Frese and Zapf
(1994) observed, high performance is not always because of
sheer effort or persistence. It is also because of cognitive
understanding of the tasks. This is likely imperative in an
unstable economic environment. A specific high-learning
goal, unlike a performance or do your best goal, may increase
the probability that a correct process or procedure will be
discovered. Thus, the first hypothesis tested was,
Hypothesis 1: The relationship between learning goals
for employees and their department’s performance
is significantly higher than that for a specific per-
formance or a vague “do-your-best” goal.
Goal commitment is defined as one’s determination to
attain the goal (Locke & Latham, 1990; Locke, Latham, &
Erez, 1988). Thus, the commitment a leader elicits from
employees to a department’s goals is critical for high perfor-
mance. Klein, Wesson, Hollenbeck, and Alge (1999) found a
direct effect of commitment to a performance goal on per-
formance. Seijts and Latham (2011), using a complex busi-
ness simulation, also found a direct effect of commitment to
a learning goal on performance. This is because “It is virtu-
ally axiomatic that a goal a person is not really trying for is
not really a goal and therefore cannot have much effect on
subsequent action” (Locke & Latham, 1990, p. 124).
As a result of this research conducted in laboratory set-
tings, where the dependent variable was an individual’s per-
formance, the following hypothesis was tested in the
workplace:
Hypothesis 2: There is a significant, positive relation-
ship between employee goal commitment and a
department’s performance.
In summary, the two following hypotheses were tested.
First, the relationship between employee learning goals and
the employees’ departmental performance is significantly
higher than that for a specific employee performance goal
or a vague employee goal, namely to a “do-your-best” goal.
Second, the relationship between employee commitment to
a learning goal and a department’s performance is signifi-
cant and positive.
Method
Sample and Procedure
A survey was conducted on three different types of
employee goals that were being set by leaders in industry
(e.g., financial, technology, and manufacturing) in the
southeastern United States. The survey was administered to
404 leaders and their employees. The survey was com-
pleted anonymously.
Of the 404 leaders with 5 or more subordinates con-
tacted, 174 and 5 of their employees (n = 870) responded
(43.1% response rate for managers and 5 employees).
1
Their respective mean ages were 37.12 years (SD = 11.17)
and 29.37 years (SD = 8.22). Their respective tenure with
their employing organization was 17.1 years (SD = 6.10)
and 3.02 years (SD = 2.69).
Measures
Goal type. The employees responded to a 6-item, 5-point
Likert-type questionnaire adapted from Seijts et al. (2004)
and Seijts and Latham (2005) for assessing learning (e.g.,
“goals set for me by my manager are based on specific
learning objectives, such as gaining knowledge or learning
a new skill”), performance (e.g., “. . . are based on specific
performance outcomes or results I need to achieve”), or
goals that are vague/abstract (e.g., “. . . are most likely to be
‘do-your-best’ goals rather than specific goals”).
The items were factor analyzed using varimax rotation.
The items loaded on three separate factors with an eigen-
value greater than 1.0, consistent with the three types of
goals. The Cronbach coefficient alphas for these three types
of goals were .82, .74, and .81, respectively.
Goal commitment. Hollenbeck and Klein (1987) noted a
large number of measurement problems in many of the
studies of goal commitment that led to inconsistent results.
Subsequently, Klein, Wesson, Hollenbeck, Wright, and
DeShon (2001) developed a reliable and valid self-report
scale for measuring goal commitment. This 5-point Likert-
type scale was used in the present study. A factor analysis
revealed that all items loaded above 0.60 on a single factor
with an eigenvalue of 2.03. The Cronbach coefficient alpha
was .78. Sample items include the following: “I am very
committed to completing the goals given to me by my man-
ager” and “I work hard to complete the goals given to me by
my manager.”
Performance. A department’s (n = 174) performance was
assessed by the respective manager on a 7-item, 5-point
Likert-type questionnaire developed by Delery and Huselid
(1998). A factor analysis revealed that all 7 items loaded
above 0.60 on a single factor with an eigenvalue of 4.04. The
Cronbach coefficient alpha was .84. Sample items include
the following: “How would you compare your department’s
performance with other departments that do the same kind of
work in terms of . . . quality of products?,” “. . .services or
performance?,” “. . . satisfaction of customers or clients?”
Controls. Age and tenure of a respondent, as well as the
size of the department and goal orientation, were controlled
in this study.
Goal orientation. Goal orientation (Dweck, 1986) is typi-
cally assessed as a trait. People with a learning-goal orienta-
tion (vs. a performance or avoiding orientation) typically
choose tasks where they can enhance their knowledge and
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Porter and Latham 65
skills. Although Seijts et al. (2004) found that goal setting
as a state masks goal orientation on a dynamic task, people
with a learning goal orientation performed better than those
with a performance goal orientation, a predisposition to
choose tasks where they can be seen as competent in the
eyes of others, in the “do-your-best” condition, weak situa-
tion (Mischel, 1969). Employees in the present study were
assessed with a 13-item, 7-point Likert-type scale devel-
oped by VandeWalle (1997). The items were factor ana-
lyzed using varimax rotation. The items loaded on three
separate factors with an eigenvalue greater than 1.0, consis-
tent with the three types of goal orientation. The Cronbach
coefficient alphas for a learning goal orientation, perfor-
mance goal orientation, and an avoiding goal orientation
were .85, .79, and .83, respectively.
Results
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics, intercorrelations,
and reliability measures for the study variables.
Tests of Hypotheses
The results of the analysis of the model are displayed in
Table 2. Regression analysis was performed for Hypotheses
1 and 2 (Aiken & West, 1991; Cohen, Cohen, West, &
Aiken, 2003).
For Hypothesis 1, effects are introduced across the col-
umns as the model is developed, indicated as numbered in
columns Model 1 and Model 2. Estimates of control vari-
ables are included in Model 1. The estimates for all three
types of employee goals are introduced in Model 2. For
Hypothesis 2, the estimate for employee goal commitment
is introduced in Model 3.
As noted earlier, each department’s performance was
determined by the supervisor on a departmental level, which
was typically composed of five employees.
2
A linear regres-
sion model was used with conditions of restricted maximum
likelihood estimation, using the SAS Version 9.1.3 and the
SAS procedure PROC.
Hypothesis 1 was supported. Learning goals explained
significantly more of the variance in a department’s perfor-
mance than did a performance or “do-your-best” goal. The
results for Model 2, shown in Table 2, indicate that the main
effect of learning goals was positive and significant (β =
0.25, p < .01). The performance goal effect, and do-your-
best goal effect were not significant (β = 0.02, p = ns) and
(β = 0.13, p = ns), respectively.
The results of this study also provide support for
Hypothesis 2. The hypothesized relationship of employee
goal commitment to departmental performance was found
to be significant and positive (β = 0.18, p < .05). In keeping
with the concept of testing the full model of interest, all the
goal type variables were kept in the regression analysis. It is
noteworthy that the effect of learning goals remained sig-
nificant and positive (β = 0.24, p < .01).
Discussion
Yukl and Mahsud (2010) argued that flexible, adaptive
leadership is essential for organizational effectiveness
when times are uncertain. This adaptation may be largely
dependent on the clarity and accuracy of the information
the leader receives, and the leaders correct interpretation
of the implications for their department’s performance. The
goals leaders subsequently set for their respective teams
should reflect this interpretation. The goals serve as the
guided motivation the leaders strive to impart to their orga-
nization’s employees.
As argued in this study, the type of goal used to motivate
employees can take several forms. The results of this study
suggest that a goal designed to focus an employee on learn-
ing the processes and procedures needed by the employee to
achieve the goal is significantly related to a department’s
Table 1. Summary Statistics and Zero-Order Correlations
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Department Performance 3.98 0.59
2. Employee Learning Goal (LG) 3.54 0.49 .27*** (.72)
3. Employee Performance Goal (PG) 3.61 0.49 .21** .59*** (.74)
4. Employee Do-Your- Best Goal (BG) 2.89 0.61 .09 .07 .20** (.71)
5. Employee Goal Commitment (GC) 3.97 0.45 .26*** .30*** .52*** .33*** (.78)
6. Employee Learning Goal Orientation 3.91 0.50 .21** .31*** .40*** .23** .70*** (.78)
7. Employee Proving Goal Orientation 3.52 0.50 .14
.23** .19* .23** .015* .28*** (.82)
8. Employee Avoiding Goal Orientation 2.92 0.69 .00 .14
.00 .50*** .35*** .27*** .43*** (.72)
9. Department Size 17.9 29.3 .01 .05 .06 .02 .15
.10 .02 .03
10. Supervisor Tenure 6.10 6.70 .04 .07 .02 .10 .07 .08 .03 .12* .04
11. Employee Tenure 3.02 2.69 .03 .09 .02 .07 .08 .01 .03 .03 .03 .01
Note. N = 174 for supervisors, 870 for employees. Reliability (α) estimates are listed on the diagonal in parentheses.
p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from
66 Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies 20(1)
performance. Defining learning goals may take more fore-
thought on the part of the leader than setting other types of
goals. For instance, performance goals may be the tradi-
tional type of goals used by an organization. In the absence
of a clearly set goal, the default unspoken goal is typically
interpreted as a vague “do-your-best” goal. Therefore, to
achieve the benefits argued for in this study, a leader needs
to consciously define employee goals in terms of discover-
ing processes and procedures to implement to improve a
department’s performance.
For instance, a traditional performance goal for a loan
officer in a bank might be stated as, “Increase commercial
loan production by 20% this quarter compared with last
quarter.” A learning goal for another loan officer might be,
“Determine five processes to increase commercial loan
production.”
This study also evaluated the effect of employee com-
mitment to the goals they were assigned. In a simulation
experiment, Seijts and Latham (2011) found that an assigned
learning goal and commitment to the goal were both signifi-
cantly related to student performance in the simulation.
These findings suggest that the assignment of a learning
goal, and commitment to that goal, act independently in
regard to a department’s performance. This finding suggests
that leaders should focus on both a learning goal and the
commitment by employees to the goals as separate actions.
That is, it is not enough to presume that assigning a learning
goal will translate into a commitment to that goal.
One way leaders can increase employee commitment to
goals is to take into account findings by Klein et al. (1999).
They reported there are strong positive overall relationships
between goal commitment and the antecedents of attrac-
tiveness of goal attainment, expectancy of goal attainment,
and motivational force. Locke et al. (1988) found that peo-
ple must understand the logic and rationale for why a goal
is set before they will commit to it.
Summary
The contribution of the present findings to the goal-setting
literature is at least fourfold. First, this is the only study to
assess the relationship of a learning goal to performance in
industrial organizations. Second, this is the only study to
examine the relationship of having one or more of three
goals, namely learning, performance, and “do-your-best”
with departmental performance. Third, this is the first study
to examine the relationship of learning goals with a macro
rather than a micro performance variable, namely, a depart-
ment’s performance rather than an individual’s. Fourth, the
present study integrated the motivational effects of goal
setting with previous research findings on leadership,
namely affecting employee goal commitment. In doing so,
the role of goal commitment on departmental performance
was examined as a direct effect.
These findings are of practical as well as theoretical
importance for the following reasons. First, the results show
that two types of goals, namely exhortations by a leader for
employees to “do-your-best” and setting a performance goal
were not significantly related to a department’s performance.
The fact that a “do-your-best” and a and a performance goal
were not related to performance likely reflects the turbulent
economic environment when this study was conducted.
There was extensive television coverage of people losing
their jobs because of companies shutting the doors and going
out of business. Hence, it is likely that employees were
uncertain as to what to do to ensure their department contrib-
uted to the employing organization’s survival.
Second, the relationship between a learning goal and a
department’s performance was high in this study. Again, this
may reflect the environmental turbulence when the study
was conducted. Falling back on extant knowledge and skills
to attain specific high performance goals may have been
necessary but not sufficient for the survival of many of those
companies. Hence, employees and their managers were
likely searching for new strategies/procedures that would
enhance their department’s competitiveness/survival.
Arguably, an important finding was the examination of a
commonly suggested variable in goal-setting theory, that is,
commitment to the assigned goal. The finding that this vari-
able acted independent from the assigned goal suggests that
it deserves considerable attention by leaders. This suggests
that leaders should spend adequate time understanding how
Table 2. Results of Regression Analysis for Hypothesis 1,
Hypothesis 2, and Hypothesis 3
a
Predictor Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
Controls
Department size (no. of
people)
0.01 0.01 0.01
Supervisor tenure (years) 0.04 0.03 0.02
Employee education (years) 0.03 0.02 0.03
Employee tenure (years) 0.07 0.02 0.02
Learning goal orientation 0.13 0.11 0.10
Performance goal
orientation
0.08 0.03 0.03
Avoiding goal orientation 0.02 0.01 0.01
Main effects
Employee learning goal (LG) 0.25** 0.24**
Employee performance goal
(PG)
0.02 0.06
Employee do-your-best goal
(BG)
0.13 0.08
Employee goal commitment
(GC)
0.18*
Adjusted R
2
.01 .05 .07
Change in adjusted R
2
.04 .03
a. Standardized coefficients are reported.
p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Porter and Latham 67
to motivate their employees to commit to assigned goals in
addition to the strategic intent of the organization when they
derive the goals.
Limitations and Future Research
Ideally, the present data, collected in a turbulent worldwide
economic crisis, would have been compared with data col-
lected in a relatively stable environment. No such data
exist. Hence, this study should be replicated if and when
relative economic stability is experienced in the United
States and elsewhere.
Determining the direction of causality among the vari-
ables is not possible with correlational data. Perhaps high-
performing departments are those who set high learning goals.
And high-performing departments may engender goal com-
mitment from employees. Despite the inability to draw casual
inferences, the present results, viewed in conjunction with the
six laboratory experiments and the field experiment in an edu-
cational setting that preceded it, suggest that organizations, in
times of uncertainty, consider setting specific learning goals
for their department, and taking steps to motivate goal com-
mitment for their respective department’s employees.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article:
This study was funded in part by a grant to Gary P. Latham by the
Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council, Canada.
Notes
1. Managers with five or more employees in their department
were surveyed. A response was considered complete when a
manager survey and five corresponding employee surveys
were completed.
2. This type of relationship, namely, multiple employees in one
department, represents nested or multilevel data. This is a form of
multilevel data that is sometimes best handled with a mixed effect
model, also referred to as a random coefficient model (RCM) or
hierarchical linear model (HLM). Therefore, a mixed effect model
with conditions of restricted maximum likelihood estimation, using
the SAS version 9.1.3 and the SAS procedure PROC MIXED. The
fit of this model was compared using the SAS procedure PROC.
No additional insight was provided using the RCM approach;
therefore, regression was used for the sake of parsimony.
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing
and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied
multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral
sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Delery, J. E., & Huselid, M. A. (1998). Issues of fit in strategic
human resource management: Implications for research.
Human Resource Management Review, 8, 289-310.
Drach-Zahavy, A., & Erez, M. (2002). Challenge versus threat
effects on the goal performance relationship. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 687-682.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning.
American Psychologist, 41, 1040-1048.
Freeland, C. (2009, April 11-April 12). Calmer Obama ushers out
the age of the imperial chief. Financial Times, p9.
Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1994). Action as the core of work psychol-
ogy: A German approach. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette,
& L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organiza-
tional psychology (pp. 271-340). Palto Alto, CA: Consulting
Psychologist Press.
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Effort and cognitive abili-
ties: An integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach
to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 5,
237-264.
Kaplan, E., Erez, M., & Van-Dijk, D. (2004, April). Reconcil-
ing differences between the goal setting and self-regulation
theories. In M. Erez (Chair), Goal setting, goal orientation,
and self regulation forms: An integration. Symposium pre-
sented at the 19th annual meeting of the Society for Industrial-
Organizational Psychology. Chicago, IL.
Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., & Alge, G. J. (1999).
Goal commitment and the goal-setting process: Conceptual
clarification and empirical synthesis, Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 84, 885-896.
Klein, H. J., Wesson, M. J., Hollenbeck, J. R., Wright, P. M., &
DeShon, R. P. (2001). The assessment of goal commitment: A
measurement model meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior
and Human Decision Processes, 85, 32-55.
Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Bell, B. S. (2006). Disentangling
achievement orientation and goal setting effects on self-
regulatory processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91,
900-916.
Latham, G. P. (2004). The motivational benefits of goal-setting.
Academy of Management Executive, 18, 126-129.
Latham, G. P., & Brown, T. C. (2006). The effect of learning vs.
outcome goals on self-efficacy, satisfaction, and performance
in an MBA program. Applied Psychology: An International
Review, 55, 606-623.
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (2007). New developments and
directions for goal-setting research, European Psychologist,
12, 290-300.
Latham, G. P., Seijts, G. H., & Crim, D. (2008). The effects of
learning goal difficulty level and cognitive ability on strategy
development and performance. Canadian Journal of Behav-
ioural Sciences, 40, 220-229.
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from
68 Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies 20(1)
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and
task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically useful
theory of goal setting and task effort. American Psychologist,
9, 705-717.
Locke, E. A., Latham, G. P., & Erez, M. (1988). The determinants of
goal commitment. Academy of Management Review, 13, 23-29.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessments. New York, NY: Wiley.
Mitchell, T. R., & Daniels, D. (2003). Motivation. In W. C. Borman,
D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Comprehensive handbook
of psychology: Industrial-organizational psychology (Vol. 12,
pp. 225-254). New York, NY: Wiley.
Mone, M. A., & Shalley, C. E. (1995). Effects of task complexity
and goal specificity on change in strategy and performance
over time. Human Performance, 8, 243-262.
Noel, T. W., & Latham, G. P. (2006). The importance of learning
versus outcome goals for entrepreneurs. International Journal
of Entrepreneurship and Innovation, 7, 213-220.
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2001). The effect of learning,
outcome, and proximal goals on a moderately complex task.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22, 291-307.
Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2005). Learning versus perfor-
mance goals: When should each be used? Academy of Man-
agement Executive, 19, 124-131.
Seijts, G. H., Latham, G. P., Tasa, K., & Latham, B. W. (2004). Goal
setting and goal orientation: An integration of two different yet
related literatures, Academy of Management Journal, 47, 227-239.
VandeWalle, D. M. (1997). Development and validation of a work
domain goal orientation instrument. Educational and Psycho-
logical Measurement, 57, 995-1015.
Winters, D., & Latham, G. P. (1996). The effect of learning versus
outcome goals on simple versus a complex task. Group and
Organization Management, 21, 235-250.
Wood, R., & Locke, E. A. (1990). Goal setting and strategy effects
on complex tasks. In B. Staw & L. Cummings (Eds.), Research
in organizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 73-109). Greenwich,
CT: JAI Press.
Yukl, G. (2012). Leadership in organizations (8th ed.). Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.
Yukl, G., & Mahsud, R. (2010). Why flexible and adaptive
leadership is essential. Consulting Psychology Journal, 62,
81-93.
Bios
Robert L. Porter is a visiting lecturer with the University of
Central Florida, College of Business in the Department of
Management. His areas of research include leadership, entrepre-
neurship, and strategy. He is a former founding director and Chief
Operating Officer for a community bank, a Chief Technology
Officer for a successful high-technology startup, and currently
consults as a Director for Management Insights.
Gary P. Latham is the Secretary of State Research Professor in
the Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto. He is
a past president of the Canadian Psychological Association and
the Society for Industrial-Organizational Psychology; he is
President-elect of Division 1, Work and Organizational
Psychology of the International Association for Applied
Psychology. He serves on the Board of the Society for Human
Resource Management.
at UNIV TORONTO on September 8, 2014jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from